Publication details
Acquisition of object-robbing and object/food-bartering behaviours: a culturally maintained token economy in free-ranging long-tailed macaques
2021

Jean‐Baptiste Leca; Noëlle Gunst; Matthew Gardiner, and others

2021
Headline

This study examines the development of token-robbing and token/reward-bartering interactions in Balinese long-tailed macaques, finding that these behaviors are learned throughout juvenescence and that older monkeys preferentially select more valued tokens and make economic decisions based on token value.

Highlights

The token exchange paradigm is an appealing and heuristically powerful system used to investigate the existence of economic behaviour in non-human primates and to explore the evolutionary origins and developmental pathways of human monetary systems

The token exchange paradigm shows that monkeys and great apes are able to use objects as symbolic tools to request specific food rewards

Around the Uluwatu Temple in Bali, Indonesia, a large free-ranging population of long-tailed macaques (Macaca fascicularis) spontaneously and routinely engage in token-mediated bartering interactions with humans. These interactions occur in two phases: after stealing inedible and more or less valuable objects from temple visitors, the macaques appear to use them as tokens, by returning them to humans in exchange for a certain number/type of food rewards proffered by the temple staff [10]. These naturally occurring token-robbing and token/reward-bartering interactions are reminiscent of the token exchange paradigm experimentally implemented by researchers in captive settings, in which the symbolic value of a token lies in the quantity and quality of the food reward gained in return [2,8,9,10,11,12,13,14,15]

During successful token/reward-bartering sequences, we found statistically significant age differences in the average number of food rewards being proposed to the monkey (H2 = 15.1, p = 0.001), the average number of food rewards being rejected by the monkey (H2 = 13.9, p = 0.001) and the average number of food rewards being rejected by the monkey before accepting a different type of food reward to end the token/reward-bartering sequence (H2 = 8.7, p = 0.013)

The present naturalistic research setting represents a unique opportunity to study field economics and explore macroeconomic phenomena in non-human primates in environmental conditions that are more externally and ecologically valid than those provided by the traditional token exchange paradigm

We found a significant increase in token-robbing success from juveniles to subadults to adults; Prediction 1a was supported

Further experimental research on the Uluwatu macaques should make future cross-species comparisons of economic decision-making and symbolic tool use more relevant from an evolutionary perspective and may lead to a better understanding of the origins of autonomous monetary systems in humans [29]

Synopsis
Summary

Snapshot

This study examines the development of token-robbing and token/reward-bartering interactions in Balinese long-tailed macaques, finding that these behaviors are learned throughout juvenescence and that older monkeys preferentially select more valued tokens and make economic decisions based on token value.

Key findings

The study found age differences in robbing/bartering success, indicative of experiential learning, and clear behavioural associations between value-based token possession and quantity or quality of food rewards rejected and accepted by subadult and adult monkeys, suggestive of robbing/bartering payoff maximization and economic decision-making.

The study finds that the monkeys exhibit experiential learning and decision-making processes in their token-robbing and token/reward-bartering behaviors. The monkeys learn to select high-valued tokens and negotiate with humans for food rewards. The study also identifies individual differences in token selection and food reward preferences among the monkeys.

The study found that token-robbing and token/reward-bartering interactions are learned throughout juvenescence, and that older monkeys preferentially select more valued tokens and make economic decisions based on token value. Specifically, the study found that: (i) token-robbing success increases from juveniles to subadults to adults; (ii) older monkeys preferentially select tokens that are more valued by humans; and (iii) more skilful and selective individuals make economic decisions based on token value.

The study found that subadult and adult monkeys preferentially selected high- and medium-valued tokens over low-valued ones, and that they strategically responded to differentially valued tokens by adjusting the amount or type of food rewards they gained from the barters.

Further experimental research on the Uluwatu macaques should make future cross-species comparisons of economic decision-making and symbolic tool use more relevant from an evolutionary perspective and may lead to a better understanding of the origins of autonomous monetary systems in humans [29]

Objectives

The objectives of the study are to understand the experiential learning and decision-making processes of long-tailed macaques in their token-robbing and token/reward-bartering behaviors, and to examine individual differences in token selection and food reward preferences.

Read more

Methods

The study used field observational and experimental data to investigate the cognitive and behavioral mechanisms underlying token-mediated bartering interactions in naturalistic circumstances.

The study uses observational data collected over 273 days, with video recordings of token-robbing and token/reward-bartering interactions between monkeys and humans. The data were collected using a combination of "behaviour-dependent sampling" and "continuous recording" rules. The study also employs experimental data from value-based token selection tests administered to a subset of 15 monkeys.

The study used a combination of observational and experimental methods to examine token-robbing and token/reward-bartering interactions in Balinese long-tailed macaques. The methods included Kruskal-Wallis H-tests, Mann-Whitney U-tests, Spearman rank-order correlation tests, Friedman tests, Wilcoxon signed-rank tests, and binomial tests.

The study used observational data and experimental methods to examine the token selection and payoff maximization behaviors of the long-tailed macaques.

Read more

Results

The study found that the monkeys exhibited token-mediated bartering interactions with humans, with age differences in robbing/bartering success and clear behavioural associations between value-based token possession and quantity or quality of food rewards rejected and accepted by subadult and adult monkeys.

The study finds that the monkeys exhibit experiential learning and decision-making processes in their token-robbing and token/reward-bartering behaviors. The results show that the monkeys learn to select high-valued tokens and negotiate with humans for food rewards. The study also identifies individual differences in token selection and food reward preferences among the monkeys.

The study found significant age differences in token-robbing success, with subadults and adults being more successful than juveniles. The study also found that older monkeys preferentially select more valued tokens and make economic decisions based on token value.

The results of the study showed that subadult and adult monkeys preferentially selected high- and medium-valued tokens over low-valued ones, and that they strategically responded to differentially valued tokens by adjusting the amount or type of food rewards they gained from the barters.

Read more

Conclusions

The study provides insights into the cognitive underpinnings of economic behaviour in non-human primates and sheds light on the evolution of human monetary systems.

The study concludes that long-tailed macaques in Uluwatu Temple exhibit complex cognitive abilities in their token-robbing and token/reward-bartering behaviors, including experiential learning and decision-making processes. The study highlights the importance of understanding individual differences in token selection and food reward preferences among the monkeys.

The study concludes that token-robbing and token/reward-bartering interactions are learned throughout juvenescence, and that older monkeys preferentially select more valued tokens and make economic decisions based on token value. The study also suggests that the development of these behaviors is influenced by experiential learning and physical maturation.

The study concludes that the long-tailed macaques exhibited economic decision-making abilities, including value-based token selection and payoff maximization, and that these abilities are age-related.

Read more
Key terms

non human primate

economic behaviour

token economy

economy

Macaca fascicularis

Dig deeper

Explore original text

Abstract Notes And Records Nengah Wandia Nengah Wandia Nengah Wandia Introduction References
Study features
Type

prospective

Participants

333 individuals

Snapshot methods

Kruskal–Wallis

Mann–Whitney U

Spearman rank-order correlation test

Friedman

Wilcoxon signed-rank

one-tailed

SPSS

Kruskal–Wallis test

Mann–Whitney test

Spearman

correlation coefficient

Friedman test

χ2

Wilcoxon

Wilcoxon test

Snapshot results

Found a statistically significant age difference in the percentage of successful token/reward-bartering attempts

Did not find a significant increase in token-robbing success from juveniles to subadults to adults, Prediction 1b was partly supported owing to the difference between the former and the latter two age classes

Snapshot conclusions

Setting represents a unique opportunity to study field economics and explore macroeconomic phenomena in non-human primates in environmental conditions that are more externally and ecologically valid than those provided by the traditional token exchange paradigm

Further experimental research on the Uluwatu macaques should make future cross-species comparisons of economic decision-making and symbolic tool use more relevant from an evolutionary perspective and may lead to a better understanding of the origins of autonomous monetary systems in humans

Evidence
score

6.0

strength

Fair

details

333 individuals; provides data. This research has been cited 15 times.

Research quality

333 individuals; provides data. This research has been cited 15 times.

Research comparison

Confirms results from previous studies

Limitations

The study notes that the ecological validity of laboratory-based experimental situations tends to be limited, and that the actual impact of conducting these experiments in artificial conditions is unknown and needs to be investigated.

The study acknowledges that the data collection method did not provide absolute behavioral frequencies due to the large study site and the difficulty of recording all robbing/bartering occurrences at any time.

The study does not discuss any limitations.

The study did not explicitly examine certain cognitive skills and temperamental traits, such as preference transitivity, self-control, delay of gratification, action planning, and calculated reciprocity, that may facilitate or constrain an individual's ability to make optimal economic decisions.

Read more

Future work

The study suggests that future research should investigate more externally and ecologically valid systems of economic behaviours in non-human primates, and critically examine the generalizability of findings from laboratory models.

The study does not suggest any future work.

Future research should examine the cognitive skills and temperamental traits mentioned above, as well as explore macroeconomic phenomena in non-human primates in environmental conditions that are more externally and ecologically valid than those provided by the traditional token exchange paradigm.

Read more

Practical applications

The study has implications for our understanding of the evolution of human monetary systems and the cognitive underpinnings of economic behaviour in non-human primates.

The study's findings have implications for our understanding of primate cognition and behavior, and may inform strategies for managing human-primate conflicts in tourist areas.

The study does not discuss any practical applications.

Read more

Subjects and statistics

Study compliance

Comparative analysis
Detail

Confirmation of earlier findings

Methods

To measure intra-coder reliability, N.G. transcribed twice a total of 6.2 h of video-recordings, involving 158 token-robbing attempts and 66 token/reward-bartering attempts, which represents 3% of the total numbers of token-robbing attempts and token/reward-bartering attempts included in the analyses of token-robbing success and token/reward-bartering success, respectively. The comparison of the two transcriptions for all the other variables analyzed in this study yielded a high score of coder consistency (mean Cohen's κ coefficient: k = 0.89 ± 0.05 [19])

Discussion

Subadult and adult monkeys (but not juveniles yet) have learned to map their token-robbing behaviours onto the hierarchical (and arbitrary) scale of values attributed by humans to different tokens: they preferentially selected tokens that were more likely to be exchanged for food (e.g. electronic devices, pairs of glasses) over other objects that were less valuable for humans and typically not worth bartering (e.g. empty camera bags, hairpins). Our findings are consistent with data obtained in other non-human primate species, showing that subadult and adult capuchin monkeys and chimpanzees correctly preferred a high-valued token over a low-valued token in an experimental bartering situation [1,11,23]

Discussion

The result showing that subadults (unlike adults) failed to significantly reject more low-preferred food rewards before returning a higher-valued token may be explained in terms of poorer temporal cognition or higher impulsivity (compared to adults): subadult long-tailed macaques may either not have yet acquired the cognitive capacity to anticipate the subsequent proffering of more preferred food rewards in this specific situation, or not be patient/self-controlled enough to wait for possibly more preferred food rewards. Overall, our field observational data are in line with laboratory-based studies showing that several non-human primate species can (i) understand the effectiveness of tokens as secondary reinforcements to make simple calculations about quantities of reward, (ii) determine an item's value on the basis of its perceived utility (e.g. exchanging only a low-preferred reward for a tool necessary to reach a more preferred reward) and (iii) recognize the appropriate conditions in which a successful exchange could occur (e.g. presence/absence of the experimenter, safe/risky experimenter) [1,2,23,24,25,26,27]

Study subjects and statistics
Participants

The study population comprised 333 individuals of long-tailed macaques (Macaca fascicularis) in the Uluwatu Temple in Bali, Indonesia, split up into five neighbouring social groups with overlapping home ranges.

individuals: 333

A population of long-tailed macaques has lived in this anthropogenic habitat for decades [17]. In April 2016, the study population comprised 333 individuals (55 adult males (older than 6 years), 21 subadult males (4–6 years), 79 adult females (older than 3.5 years), 20 subadult females (2.5–3.5 years), 94 juveniles (1–4 years for males and 1–2.5 for females) and 64 infants (younger than 1 year) split up into five neighbouring social groups with overlapping home ranges [16]. The Uluwatu macaques were provisioned daily with a variety of fruits and vegetables provided by the temple staff, and fully habituated to human presence

samples: 390

Because the monkeys were highly habituated to humans, most video-recordings were collected at close range (2–5 m) without disturbing the animals. Data collection started when the two observers reached high inter-observer reliability, as measured by the index of concordance (C) for monkey identities (N = 390 samples, C = 0.96) and age–sex class identification (N = 350, C = 0.93), and distances in metres (N = 200, C = 0.91) [16,19]. Even though these environmental conditions and behavioural sampling techniques allow for a majority of the token-robbing and token/reward-bartering interactions to be accurately and consistently recorded from start to finish, they did not provide absolute behavioural frequencies (i.e. number of events/behaviours per observation time unit) because the study site was too large for all robbing/bartering occurrences to be recorded at any time

Statistics

To test age differences in the percentage of successful token-robbing attempts, the percentage of successful token/reward-bartering attempts, and the average number of food rewards being proposed and rejected by the monkey, we used Kruskal–Wallis H-tests followed, when significant, by post hoc Mann–Whitney U-tests

To test whether the monkeys selected the six different types of tokens based on their relative local availability, we used a Spearman rank-order correlation test between the numbers of the six types of tokens available from our sample of 500 potential human targets and the numbers of the six types of tokens selected by the monkeys within our entire set of observational data (i.e. the sum of Nmissed and Ntaken for each type of token)

To test within-individual preferential selection of low-valued, medium-valued and high-valued tokens, we used Friedman tests followed, when significant, by post hoc Wilcoxon signed-rank tests

Because all our predictions were directional, we conducted one-tailed tests

Statistical analyses were performed using the IBM-SPSS Statistics-26 analytical program

We found a statistically significant age difference in the percentage of successful token-robbing attempts (Kruskal–Wallis test, H2 = 35.8, p < 0.001)

p = 0.002

We found a statistically significant age difference in the percentage of successful token-robbing attempts (Kruskal–Wallis test, H2 = 35.8, p < 0.001). Post hoc pairwise comparisons revealed that subadults were more successful than juveniles (Mann–Whitney test, Nsubadult = 20, Njuvenile = 17, U = 69.0, p = 0.002), adults were more successful than juveniles (Nadult = 60, U = 68.5, p < 0.001), and adults were more successful than subadults (U = 302.5, p = 0.001; figure 1). Therefore, we found a significant increase in token-robbing success from juveniles to subadults to adults; Prediction 1a was supported

p = 0.001

We found a statistically significant age difference in the percentage of successful token/reward-bartering attempts (H2 = 22.3, p < 0.001). Post hoc pairwise comparisons revealed that subadults were more successful than juveniles (Nsubadult = 13, Njuvenile = 10, U = 9.5, p = 0.001) and adults were more successful than juveniles (Nadult = 45, U = 20.0, p < 0.001). However, adults and subadults did not significantly differ in percentages of successful token/reward-bartering attempts (U = 213.0, p = 0.135; figure 1)

p = 0.135

Post hoc pairwise comparisons revealed that subadults were more successful than juveniles (Nsubadult = 13, Njuvenile = 10, U = 9.5, p = 0.001) and adults were more successful than juveniles (Nadult = 45, U = 20.0, p < 0.001). However, adults and subadults did not significantly differ in percentages of successful token/reward-bartering attempts (U = 213.0, p = 0.135; figure 1). Even though we did not find a significant increase in token-robbing success from juveniles to subadults to adults, Prediction 1b was partly supported owing to the difference between the former and the latter two age classes

p = 0.001

Even though we did not find a significant increase in token-robbing success from juveniles to subadults to adults, Prediction 1b was partly supported owing to the difference between the former and the latter two age classes. During successful token/reward-bartering sequences, we found statistically significant age differences in the average number of food rewards being proposed to the monkey (H2 = 15.1, p = 0.001), the average number of food rewards being rejected by the monkey (H2 = 13.9, p = 0.001) and the average number of food rewards being rejected by the monkey before accepting a different type of food reward to end the token/reward-bartering sequence (H2 = 8.7, p = 0.013). In each of these three variables, subadults scored significantly higher than juveniles (Nsubadult = 14, Njuvenile = 9, U = 11.0, p < 0.001; U = 12.5, p = 0.001; U = 19.0, p = 0.004, respectively) and adults scored higher than juveniles (Nadult = 46, U = 41.5, p < 0.001; U = 49.0, p < 0.001; U = 87.5, p = 0.006, respectively)

Introduction

The token exchange paradigm is an appealing and heuristically powerful system used to investigate the existence of economic behaviour in non-human primates and to explore the evolutionary origins and developmental pathways of human monetary systems. Like a symbolic tool, a token is an inherently non-valuable (e.g. inedible or non-nutritive) object that acts as a secondary conditioned reinforcer; indeed, it only acquires an instrumental or functional value through the arbitrary associations made with the goods (e.g. food) or services (e.g. social or sexual favours) it is conventionally exchanged for in a barter-like situation, even though the object bears no iconic relation to its referent [1,2]. The tokens used by primate subjects in such experimental studies physically resemble human coins, and the act of trading tokens is similar to that of exchanging money for other commodities. The token exchange paradigm has shown that several species of monkeys and great apes can use tokens to request specific food rewards. This line of research provides insights into the cognitive underpinnings of economic behaviour in non-human primates [3].

However, most of these experimental procedures involve human-induced exchanges with relatively small samples of individually trained, laboratory-bred subjects. During the experiments, these subjects (i) are typically placed in isolation from their conspecifics and their other daily activities, (ii) exchanged in constrained environments characterized by a lack of alternative response options, and (iii) received small rewards for the correct actions ([1,2,4,5], but see [6,7,8]). These conditions markedly contrast with real-world human economic behaviours that offer many different formats and variants, often occur over extended periods of time, are spontaneously engaged in by a very heterogeneous population, use a range of symbolic currencies and are influenced by a rich social context [3,9].

In this respect, the external and ecological validities of the currently available token exchange paradigm could be put into question. This is not to say that the results obtained from the current studies suffer from a complete lack of validity, or that the results may not be informative about some contexts. However, the actual impact of conducting these experiments in these artificial conditions is unknown and needs to be investigated. One way to do that is to study more externally and ecologically valid systems of economic behaviours in non-human primates, and then to critically examine the generalizability of findings from laboratory models. This approach should provide a more solid platform for conducting comparative economics research and shed light on the evolution of human monetary systems [3].

Around the Uluwatu Temple in Bali, Indonesia, a large free-ranging population of long-tailed macaques (Macaca fascicularis) spontaneously and routinely engage in token-mediated bartering interactions with humans. These interactions occur in two phases: after stealing inedible and more or less valuable objects (e.g. pairs of glasses, hats, empty bags) from temple visitors, the macaques appear to use them as tokens, by returning them to humans in exchange for a certain number/type of food rewards proffered by the temple staff [10]. In many respects, these naturally occurring token-robbing and token/reward-bartering interactions are reminiscent of the token exchange paradigm experimentally implemented by researchers in captive settings, in which the symbolic value of a token lies in the quantity and quality of the food reward gained in return [2,8,9,10,11,12,13,14,15]. Our research project is the first to explore the cognitive and behavioural mechanisms underlying the spontaneous expression of token-mediated bartering interactions in naturalistic circumstances (i.e. the macaques initiate token-robbing interactions without any encouragement from humans). These interactions are at least partially monkey-driven (i.e. even though token/reward-bartering interactions depend on the willingness of human barterers to exchange, the macaques can choose to barter or not), and exhibited by a large number of free-ranging individuals [10].

Recent reports on robbing/bartering in the Uluwatu macaques have shown that this behaviour was (i) population-specific, because it was not observed in other anthropogenically influenced populations of Balinese macaques where it would be ecologically possible, (ii) frequent (7.8 robbing events/hour and 3.3 bartering events/hour), (iii) prevalent (49.3% of identified population members performed robbing behaviour and 35.6% performed both robbing and bartering behaviours), (iv) performed by individuals from all age/sex classes in the five neighbouring social groups of this population, (v) characterized by substantial intrapopulational intergroup variation in its frequency and prevalence (but not success), which was explained by anthropogenic influences (i.e. differential environmental opportunities to interact with temple visitors), and to a lesser extent, demographic correlates (i.e. the behaviour was more frequent and more prevalent in groups with higher ratios of (sub)adult males, but not group density), (vi) a behavioural candidate for the process of ‘cultural zones' (in which neighbouring social groups share space, information and behavioural practices) via intergroup observational learning or intergroup transfers of male group members, (vii) socially influenced and synchronized in its group-level expression, following a behavioural contagion-like effect, owing to response facilitation and possibly conformity-biased learning, (viii) not influenced by model-based biases (i.e. dominance rank, age, experience and success of the demonstrator), and (ix) an established practice in this population, probably passed on cross-generationally for at least 30 years, though detailed information on its origins is lacking [10,16]. However, we do not know whether this behaviour is learned through individual practice (i.e. via experiential trial-and-error learning) and underlain by economic decision-making processes—that is, subjected to robbing/bartering payoff maximization as would be suggested by clear behavioural associations between value-based token possession and quantity or quality of food rewards rejected and accepted by the monkeys.

This study aimed to fill this knowledge gap by testing three hypotheses pertaining to the acquisition and skillful performance of robbing and bartering interactions in the Uluwatu population of long-tailed macaques. First, the ‘experiential learning' hypothesis posits age differences in robbing/bartering success. Indeed, each of the two aforementioned phases (i.e. robbing and bartering) comprises several successive behavioural steps or strategies that must all be appropriately completed by the monkey for the phase to be considered successful. Both token-robbing and token/reward-bartering are challenging tasks for a monkey and require specific perceptual learning, sensorimotor coordination and cognitive skills. From juveniles to subadults to adults, we predicted (i) an increase in token-robbing success (i.e. the monkey sequentially stared at a temple visitor, approached and snatched a token from this person, and held the token, while stepping aside; Prediction 1a), (ii) an increase in token/reward-bartering success (i.e. the monkey waited for a human barterer and returned the undamaged token in exchange for at least one food reward; Prediction 1b), and (iii) more negotiated successful token/reward-bartering sequences (i.e. the monkey only returned the token after being proposed more food rewards, or after rejecting more food rewards, or after accepting a type of food reward different from the one(s) previously rejected; Prediction 1c).

Second, assuming a hierarchical scale of values attributed by humans to different tokens (with more valuable tokens leading to more bartering attempts by the temple staff), the ‘value-based token selection' hypothesis posits age differences in the selection of higher-valued tokens by the monkeys during the token-robbing phase. From juveniles to subadults to adults, we predicted an increase in the relative selection of higher-valued tokens available among all the temple visitors present on the site (Prediction 2a). When two tokens of different values were available on a given temple visitor, we predicted that subadult and adult monkeys would preferentially select the higher-valued token, whereas no significant difference was expected in juvenile monkeys (Prediction 2b). We predicted a similar age difference in the value-based token selection during field experiments, after controlling for token accessibility and lateral bias (Prediction 2c).

Third, assuming that economic decision-making requires cognitive maturity, the ‘robbing/bartering payoff maximization' hypothesis posits clear behavioural associations between value-based token possession and quantity or quality of food rewards rejected and accepted by subadult and adult monkeys (i.e. hypothetically the most skillful and selective individuals) during the token/reward-bartering phase. In other words, these individuals should respond differently to different token values, and use the token/reward-bartering phase to obtain either more food rewards or a more preferred food reward when they selected a higher-valued token. In terms of reward quantity, subadult and adult subjects should (i) wait to be proposed more food rewards (Prediction 3a), and (ii) accumulate more than one of the proposed food rewards (Prediction 3b) before returning the token when it is a higher-valued token. In terms of reward quality, they should (i) reject more low-preferred food rewards before returning the token when it is a higher-valued token (Prediction 3c) and (ii) more likely end a successful token/reward-bartering sequence by returning the token in exchange for a type of food reward different from the one(s) previously rejected when it is a higher-valued token (Prediction 3d). Conversely, subadult and adult subjects should more likely end a successful token/reward-bartering sequence by returning the token in exchange for a low-preferred food reward when it is a lower-valued token (Prediction 3e).

Objectives

This study aimed to fill this knowledge gap by testing three hypotheses pertaining to the acquisition and skillful performance of robbing and bartering interactions in the Uluwatu population of long-tailed macaques

Methods

The Uluwatu Temple, located in a dry agricultural landscape of southern Bali, Indonesia, is a Hindu temple complex used by Balinese communities for daily religious ceremonies. It is also one of the most famous touristic spots on the island, visited by 1.5 million tourists in 2015 [16]. A population of long-tailed macaques has lived in this anthropogenic habitat for decades [17]. In April 2016, the study population comprised 333 individuals (55 adult males (older than 6 years), 21 subadult males (4–6 years), 79 adult females (older than 3.5 years), 20 subadult females (2.5–3.5 years), 94 juveniles (1–4 years for males and 1–2.5 for females) and 64 infants (younger than 1 year) split up into five neighbouring social groups with overlapping home ranges [16]. The Uluwatu macaques were provisioned daily with a variety of fruits and vegetables provided by the temple staff, and fully habituated to human presence. Overall, this habitat is a human-dominated area characterized by highly frequent interactions with (local and tourist) people and a very low predation pressure [16].

Observational data were based on the spontaneous expression of token-robbing and token/reward-bartering interactions between monkeys and humans, with no intervention from the researchers. These data were collected by using the combined ‘behaviour-dependent sampling' rule and the ‘continuous recording' rule [18]. Behaviour-dependent sampling typically applies to data collection on ‘conspicuous' or ‘attention-attracting' behaviours [18, p.87]; these are characteristics that fit token-robbing and token/reward-bartering interactions, particularly because they usually occur within a few wide and open areas located around Uluwatu temple, where visitors are allowed and visibility for the researchers was good [10,16]. All observational data were continuously video-recorded from September 2015 to August 2016 (over 273 observation days) by two observers, using handheld camcorders (Sony HDR-PJ670). They typically walked across these open areas, regularly scanned the monkeys and their potential human targets around them, and started video-recording when a monkey stared at a prospective human target while approaching this person within 5 m. A prospective human target was defined as a temple visitor wearing or carrying around at least one inedible object that was more or less likely to be subsequently exchanged for food if stolen; examples of worn tokens included sun/eyeglasses and flip-flops, whereas examples of tokens carried around included cell phones and empty camera bags.

For each robbing/bartering event, one of the two observers video-recorded the complete token-robbing attempt and token/reward-bartering attempt, including the identity of the monkey being involved, the object (i.e. prospective token) being selected by the monkey, the number and type of other potential tokens directly accessible to the monkey (i.e. held or carried around by the human target), and the number and type of food reward(s) being (i) proposed/tossed to the monkey by an accustomed temple staff member typically located within 3 metres, (ii) ignored/rejected by the monkey with a ‘turning down' hand/arm gesture, and (iii) accepted/accumulated by the monkey by grabbing/cuddling the food reward(s). Because the monkeys were highly habituated to humans, most video-recordings were collected at close range (2–5 m) without disturbing the animals. Data collection started when the two observers reached high inter-observer reliability, as measured by the index of concordance (C) for monkey identities (N = 390 samples, C = 0.96) and age–sex class identification (N = 350, C = 0.93), and distances in metres (N = 200, C = 0.91) [16,19].

Even though these environmental conditions and behavioural sampling techniques allow for a majority of the token-robbing and token/reward-bartering interactions to be accurately and consistently recorded from start to finish, they did not provide absolute behavioural frequencies (i.e. number of events/behaviours per observation time unit) because the study site was too large for all robbing/bartering occurrences to be recorded at any time. However, this data collection method allowed us to quantify all the other variables used in this study (i.e. individual percentages of robbing/bartering successful attempts; number and type of object(s) being available on, and selected from, a human target; individual average numbers of food rewards being proposed, rejected, and accepted) because these measures were based on the relative behavioural frequencies calculated from a total of recorded events. All these variables were obtained from video-scoring. This task was conducted by N.G., who transcribed all the video-recorded robbing/bartering interactions onto an Excel spreadsheet, with each aforementioned variable being coded to the second. To measure intra-coder reliability, N.G. transcribed twice a total of 6.2 h of video-recordings, involving 158 token-robbing attempts and 66 token/reward-bartering attempts, which represents 3% of the total numbers of token-robbing attempts and token/reward-bartering attempts included in the analyses of token-robbing success and token/reward-bartering success, respectively. The comparison of the two transcriptions for all the other variables analyzed in this study yielded a high score of coder consistency (mean Cohen's κ coefficient: k = 0.89 ± 0.05 [19]).

We used individual percentages of successful token-robbing attempts and successful token/reward-bartering attempts as valid measures of experiential learning by conducting cross-sectional analyses based on age differences. To calculate these percentages, we used each monkey subject as its own control by distinguishing for each attempt two possible outcomes—successful or unsuccessful—out of the total number of attempts for each subject.

There was a four-stepped sequence of appropriate behaviours and environmental conditions leading to a successful token-robbing outcome: the monkey (i) stared at a prospective human target, (ii) inconspicuously approached the human target within 5 m, (iii) snatched the object from the human target (monkey–human body contact may or may not occur), and (iv) stepped aside while gripping the token. If any of these behavioural steps was not appropriately completed by the monkey, the token-robbing attempt was considered unsuccessful. To be included in the analyses of token-robbing outcome, an individual had to be involved in at least four token-robbing attempts (mean number of token-robbing attempts/individual ±s.d. = 53.5 ± 76.9). We believe that this minimum number of four attempts per individual made the distinction between unsuccessful and successful outcomes valid, while allowing us to reach a sample size for the ‘juvenile' age class that was conducive to statistical analysis.

Following a successful token-robbing attempt, there was a three-stepped sequence of appropriate behaviours and environmental conditions leading to a successful token/reward-bartering outcome: the monkey (i) held onto the stolen object and remained present in the area for a potential token/reward-bartering attempt involving a human barterer (i.e. typically one of the accustomed temple staff members, or occasionally the human target), instead of fleeing with an object that only acquires a value for the monkey during the bartering process, (ii) engaged in a token/reward-bartering interaction, which may involve accumulating several food rewards before returning the token, or even discarding a (a number of) less preferred food reward(s) in anticipation of receiving a (number of) more preferred food reward(s), and (iii) refrained itself from damaging the token and returned it in good condition, because if the token was damaged by the monkey, the bartering interaction may not occur or may be interrupted. If any of these behavioural steps was not appropriately completed by the monkey, the token/reward-bartering attempt was considered unsuccessful. To be included in the analyses of token/reward-bartering success, an individual had to be involved in at least four token/reward-bartering attempts (mean number of token/reward-bartering attempts/individual ± s.d. = 31.6 ± 39.0).

Our observational data showed six types of tokens selected (i.e. either missed, if the token-robbing attempt was unsuccessful, or taken, if it was successful) by the Uluwatu monkeys: (i) empty containers (e.g. phone cases, camera bags, plastic bottles; Nmissed = 260, Ntaken = 287); (ii) accessories (e.g. hairpins, keyrings, bag-charms; Nmissed = 171, Ntaken = 270); (iii) hats (e.g. headgear, caps, crowns, veils, scarves; Nmissed = 181, Ntaken = 257); (iv) shoes (e.g. flip-flops, heeled sandals; Nmissed = 897, Ntaken = 902); (v) pairs of glasses (e.g. eyeglasses, sunglasses; Nmissed = 351, Ntaken = 1773); and (vi) electronic devices/wallets (e.g. cell phones, cameras, tablets, purses; Nmissed = 48, Ntaken = 77). Edible objects selected by the monkeys were not considered as tokens in our analyses.

To establish a hierarchical scale of values assigned by humans to these six types of tokens, we ranked them by increasing percentages of successful token-robbing attempts leading to token/reward-bartering attempts by humans (i.e. assuming more human-valued tokens should lead to more token/reward-bartering attempts by humans to retrieve the tokens from the monkeys). Our observational data showed three groups (each containing two types) of value-based tokens on the basis of how often humans wanted to engage in token/reward-bartering interactions with the monkeys: (i) low-valued tokens, seldom bartered by humans: empty containers and accessories (leading to bartering attempts in 8.4% and 16.3% of events, respectively); (ii) medium-valued tokens, often bartered by humans: hats and shoes (leading to bartering attempts in 74.3% and 75.8% of events, respectively); and (iii) high-valued tokens, almost always bartered by humans: pairs of glasses and electronic devices/wallets (leading to bartering attempts in 90.6% and 98.7% of events, respectively).

To assess the relative local availability in the six different types of tokens, we examined a sample of 84 video-recorded token-robbing events, randomly selected from our observational data, that featured 500 potential human targets. For each of these potential human targets, we scored the number and the type of potential tokens directly accessible to the monkeys. From this representative sample, the total number of available tokens we obtained was N = 1084, distributed across the six types of tokens as follows: (i) 127 empty containers; (ii) 107 accessories; (iii) 132 hats; (iv) 466 (pairs of) shoes; (v) 120 pairs of glasses; and (vi) 132 electronic devices/wallets. To assess the relative ease with which the six different types of tokens were stolen by the monkeys from human targets, we calculated, for each type of token, the average percentage of successful token-robbing attempts among all the subadult and adult monkeys (i.e. the most skilled robbers) that had each been involved in at least four token-robbing attempts targeting a given type of token in our observational dataset.

Experimental data were obtained from value-based token selection tests administered to a subset of 15 monkeys belonging to the three age classes (i.e. five juvenile, five subadult and five adult individuals) by three experimenters between September and December 2019. When each of these subjects was not surrounded by other monkeys, it was presented with a binary choice between a medium-valued token and a high-valued token, each proffered at opposing sides of a black plastic tray (30 cm × 20 cm × 4 cm). The tray was placed on the ground at 1.2 m from the tested monkey. The preferentially selected token was deemed to be the one the subject (first) grasped/took possession of. Each subject was tested across 16 trials comprising four randomly rotated sessions of four randomly rotated binary choices between a medium-valued token and a high-valued token (i.e. four trials with hat versus pairs of glasses, four trials with hat versus phone, four trials with shoe versus pairs of glasses and four trials with shoe versus phone). To control for lateral biases in grasping behaviour, token orientation (i.e. left/right) was balanced across trials. To control for experimenter biases, the experimenters fixed their gaze at a fixed point away from the proffered tokens during each trial. Most of these trials were followed by token/reward-bartering attempts by experimenters to retrieve the tokens. In terms of timing, and as much as possible considering field study constraints, the trials were homogeneously distributed within and across subjects, with no more than five trials being performed on a given subject within a day. Experimental data collection started when the three experimenters reached 100% of concordance for monkey identities.

For analyses on food rewards rejected and accepted by the monkeys during the token/reward-bartering phase, we only considered the food items that the monkey could reach/grab when proposed/tossed to them by the human barterers. To assess individual preferences across the three types of proposed food rewards (i.e. a raw egg, a small plastic bag containing a few pieces of fruit and a cracker), we used our observational data on spontaneously expressed token/reward-bartering interactions.

For a given monkey, we calculated three ‘food reward likeability' scores, one for each of the three types of proposed food rewards. When the three reward types were proposed during a token/reward-bartering sequence, we used two values: X was defined as the total number of times a certain reward type was the last one accepted by the monkey before returning the token and Y was defined as the total number of times this reward type was rejected by the monkey during token/reward-bartering sequences. The individual ‘likeability' score for a reward type was obtained by dividing X by (X + Y). The least-preferred type of food rewards for a given monkey was considered to be the one with the lowest ‘likeability' score.

Because all our raw and (log, square-root and arsine-square-root) transformed data violated parametric assumptions, we conducted nonparametric tests. To test age differences in the percentage of successful token-robbing attempts, the percentage of successful token/reward-bartering attempts, and the average number of food rewards being proposed and rejected by the monkey, we used Kruskal–Wallis H-tests followed, when significant, by post hoc Mann–Whitney U-tests. To test whether the monkeys selected the six different types of tokens based on their relative local availability, we used a Spearman rank-order correlation test between the numbers of the six types of tokens available from our sample of 500 potential human targets and the numbers of the six types of tokens selected by the monkeys within our entire set of observational data (i.e. the sum of Nmissed and Ntaken for each type of token). To test whether the monkeys preferentially selected tokens that were easier to steal from humans, we used a Spearman rank-order correlation test between the percentage of types of tokens selected and the average percentage of successful token-robbing attempts among subadult and adult monkeys. To test within-individual preferential selection of low-valued, medium-valued and high-valued tokens, we used Friedman tests followed, when significant, by post hoc Wilcoxon signed-rank tests. To test within-individual preferential selection between two tokens of different values available on a given human target, we used Wilcoxon signed-rank tests. Value-based token selection tests of binary choices between a medium-valued token and a high-valued token were analyzed by using binomial tests with a test proportion = 0.50. Because all our predictions were directional, we conducted one-tailed tests. Statistical analyses were performed using the IBM-SPSS Statistics-26 analytical program. Significance levels were set at α = 0.05.

Results

The percentages of successful token-robbing attempts were 39.1% (N = 376 attempts), 61.5% (N = 1829 attempts) and 68.8% (N = 3062 attempts) in juveniles, subadults, and adults, respectively. We found a statistically significant age difference in the percentage of successful token-robbing attempts (Kruskal–Wallis test, H2 = 35.8, p < 0.001). Post hoc pairwise comparisons revealed that subadults were more successful than juveniles (Mann–Whitney test, Nsubadult = 20, Njuvenile = 17, U = 69.0, p = 0.002), adults were more successful than juveniles (Nadult = 60, U = 68.5, p < 0.001), and adults were more successful than subadults (U = 302.5, p = 0.001; figure 1). Therefore, we found a significant increase in token-robbing success from juveniles to subadults to adults; Prediction 1a was supported.

The percentages of successful token/reward-bartering attempts were 72.4% (N = 58 attempts), 89.4% (N = 689 attempts) and 92.0% (N = 1446 attempts) in juveniles, subadults and adults, respectively. We found a statistically significant age difference in the percentage of successful token/reward-bartering attempts (H2 = 22.3, p < 0.001). Post hoc pairwise comparisons revealed that subadults were more successful than juveniles (Nsubadult = 13, Njuvenile = 10, U = 9.5, p = 0.001) and adults were more successful than juveniles (Nadult = 45, U = 20.0, p < 0.001). However, adults and subadults did not significantly differ in percentages of successful token/reward-bartering attempts (U = 213.0, p = 0.135; figure 1). Even though we did not find a significant increase in token-robbing success from juveniles to subadults to adults, Prediction 1b was partly supported owing to the difference between the former and the latter two age classes.

During successful token/reward-bartering sequences, we found statistically significant age differences in the average number of food rewards being proposed to the monkey (H2 = 15.1, p = 0.001), the average number of food rewards being rejected by the monkey (H2 = 13.9, p = 0.001) and the average number of food rewards being rejected by the monkey before accepting a different type of food reward to end the token/reward-bartering sequence (H2 = 8.7, p = 0.013). In each of these three variables, subadults scored significantly higher than juveniles (Nsubadult = 14, Njuvenile = 9, U = 11.0, p < 0.001; U = 12.5, p = 0.001; U = 19.0, p = 0.004, respectively) and adults scored higher than juveniles (Nadult = 46, U = 41.5, p < 0.001; U = 49.0, p < 0.001; U = 87.5, p = 0.006, respectively). However, adults and subadults did not significantly differ in these scores (U = 301.0, p = 0.713; U = 313.5, p = 0.882; U = 305.0, p = 0.766, respectively). Even though we did not find a significant increase in the scores indicating negotiated successful token/reward-bartering sequences from juveniles to subadults to adults, Prediction 1c was partly supported owing to the difference between the former and the latter two age classes.

In the observational data, the relative local availability in the six types of tokens and the numbers of the six types of tokens selected by the monkeys within our entire set of observational data were not statistically correlated (Spearman's correlation coefficient, N = 6, Rs = −0.17, p = 0.742). Moreover, the average percentage of successful token-robbing attempts among all the subadult and adult monkeys (i.e. the most skilled robbers) and the percentage of types of tokens selected from human targets were not statistically correlated (Spearman's correlation coefficient, N = 6, Rs = −0.26, p = 0.623).

In the observational data, the percentages of selection of low-valued, medium-valued and high-valued tokens were, respectively, 25.3, 47.9 and 26.8% in juveniles, 18.8, 38.9 and 42.3% in subadults and 17.2, 41.4 and 41.4% in adults. We did not find any statistically significant difference in the selection of the three differentially valued tokens by juveniles (Friedman test, N = 17, χ2 = 4.9, p = 0.085). However, we found statistically significant differences in the selection of the three differentially valued tokens by subadults (N = 20, χ2 = 8.5, p = 0.014) and adults (N = 60, χ2 = 21.4, p < 0.001). Post hoc pairwise comparisons revealed that subadults and adults preferentially selected high-valued over low-valued tokens (Wilcoxon tests, subadults: z = −2.0, p = 0.049; adults: z = −3.6, p < 0.001) and medium-valued over low-valued tokens (subadults: z = −2.0, p = 0.049; adults: z = −2.6, p = 0.010). There was no significant difference in the selection of high-valued or medium-valued tokens in subadults (z = −0.2, p = 0.872) and adults (z = −0.1, p = 0.997). Even though we did not find a significant increase in the relative selection of higher-valued tokens among all the prospective human targets from juveniles to subadults to adults, Prediction 2a was partly supported owing to the difference between the former and the latter two age classes.

However, when two tokens of different values were available on a given human target, we found that subadult and adult monkeys preferentially selected the higher-valued token (Wilcoxon tests, Nsubadult = 12, z = −3.0, p = 0.003; Nadult = 12, z = −3.1, p = 0.002), whereas no significant difference was found in juvenile individuals (Njuvenile = 12, z = −0.775, p = 0.439). Therefore, Prediction 2b was supported.

In the experimental data, when two tokens of different values were available on plastic trays placed in front of a tested monkey, we found that subadult and adult subjects preferentially selected the higher-valued token (Binomial tests, subadults: Nmedium-valued tokens selected = 12, Nhigh-valued tokens selected = 68, p < 0.001; adults: Nmedium-valued tokens selected = 10, Nhigh-valued tokens selected = 70, p < 0.001), whereas no significant difference was found in juvenile subjects (Nmedium-valued tokens selected = 35, Nhigh-valued tokens selected = 45, p = 0.314). Therefore, Prediction 2c was supported.

In this section about economic decision-making (i.e. testing behavioural associations between value-based token possession and quantity or quality of food rewards rejected and accepted), we focused on the most skilful and selective individuals during the token/reward-bartering phase, namely, subadult and adult monkeys. In terms of reward quantity, subadult and adult subjects waited to be proposed more food rewards before returning the token when it was a higher-valued token (i.e. a high-valued rather than a medium-valued token; Wilcoxon tests, Nsubadult = 12, z = −3.1, p = 0.002; Nadult = 40, z = −3.0, p = 0.003). Low-valued tokens could not be used in these analyses because, by definition, they were typically not bartered by the temple staff. Still, Prediction 3a was supported.

We also found that the percentage of successful token/reward-bartering sequences when more than one of the proposed food rewards were accumulated by the monkey before returning the token was significantly higher when the monkey was holding a high-valued token than when it was holding a medium-valued token, both in subadult (Wilcoxon test, N = 10, z = −2.8, p = 0.005) and adult subjects (N = 29, z = −2.4, p = 0.015). Therefore, Prediction 3b was supported.

In terms of reward quality, and as expected, the average percentage of rejections, by adult subjects, of the least-preferred type of food rewards (when at least one reward among the least-preferred type was proposed during a given token/reward-bartering sequence) was significantly higher when they were holding a high-valued token, compared to a medium-valued token (Wilcoxon test, N = 22, z = −2.3, p = 0.024). However, this difference was not statistically significant in subadult subjects (Wilcoxon test, N = 10, z = −1.4, p = 0.153). Therefore, Prediction 3c was supported for adult, but not for subadult subjects.

Contrary to our expectations, there was no significant difference in the average number of food rewards being rejected by the monkey before accepting a different type of food reward to end the token/reward-bartering sequence when it was holding a high-valued token, compared to a medium-valued token, either in subadults (Wilcoxon test, N = 9, z = −1.2, p = 0.223) or in adults (Wilcoxon test, N = 29, z = −1.1, p = 0.280). Therefore, Prediction 3d was not supported.

Finally, subadult and adult subjects were significantly more likely to end a successful token/reward-bartering sequence by returning the token in exchange for the least-preferred type of food rewards (provided at least one reward among the least-preferred type was proposed during a given token/reward-bartering sequence) when they were holding a medium-valued token compared to when they were holding a high-valued token (Wilcoxon tests, Nsubadult = 10, z = −2.2, p = 0.028; Nadult = 22, z = −2.1, p = 0.033). Therefore, Prediction 3e was supported.

Findings

Post hoc pairwise comparisons revealed that subadults were more successful than juveniles (Mann–Whitney test, Nsubadult = 20, Njuvenile = 17, U = 69.0, p = 0.002), adults were more successful than juveniles (Nadult = 60, U = 68.5, p < 0.001), and adults were more successful than subadults (U = 302.5, p = 0.001; figure 1)

We found a significant increase in token-robbing success from juveniles to subadults to adults; Prediction 1a was supported

Post hoc pairwise comparisons revealed that subadults were more successful than juveniles (Nsubadult = 13, Njuvenile = 10, U = 9.5, p = 0.001) and adults were more successful than juveniles (Nadult = 45, U = 20.0, p < 0.001)

Even though we did not find a significant increase in token-robbing success from juveniles to subadults to adults, Prediction 1b was partly supported owing to the difference between the former and the latter two age classes

During successful token/reward-bartering sequences, we found statistically significant age differences in the average number of food rewards being proposed to the monkey (H2 = 15.1, p = 0.001), the average number of food rewards being rejected by the monkey (H2 = 13.9, p = 0.001) and the average number of food rewards being rejected by the monkey before accepting a different type of food reward to end the token/reward-bartering sequence (H2 = 8.7, p = 0.013)

In each of these three variables, subadults scored significantly higher than juveniles (Nsubadult = 14, Njuvenile = 9, U = 11.0, p < 0.001; U = 12.5, p = 0.001; U = 19.0, p = 0.004, respectively) and adults scored higher than juveniles (Nadult = 46, U = 41.5, p < 0.001; U = 49.0, p < 0.001; U = 87.5, p = 0.006, respectively)

Even though we did not find a significant increase in the scores indicating negotiated successful token/reward-bartering sequences from juveniles to subadults to adults, Prediction 1c was partly supported owing to the difference between the former and the latter two age classes. (b) ‘Value-based token selection' hypothesis

We found statistically significant differences in the selection of the three differentially valued tokens by subadults (N = 20, χ2 = 8.5, p = 0.014) and adults (N = 60, χ2 = 21.4, p < 0.001)

Post hoc pairwise comparisons revealed that subadults and adults preferentially selected high-valued over low-valued tokens (Wilcoxon tests, subadults: z = −2.0, p = 0.049; adults: z = −3.6, p < 0.001) and medium-valued over low-valued tokens (subadults: z = −2.0, p = 0.049; adults: z = −2.6, p = 0.010)

There was no significant difference in the selection of high-valued or medium-valued tokens in subadults (z = −0.2, p = 0.872) and adults (z = −0.1, p = 0.997)

Even though we did not find a significant increase in the relative selection of higher-valued tokens among all the prospective human targets from juveniles to subadults to adults, Prediction 2a was partly supported owing to the difference between the former and the latter two age classes

When two tokens of different values were available on a given human target, we found that subadult and adult monkeys preferentially selected the higher-valued token (Wilcoxon tests, Nsubadult = 12, z = −3.0, p = 0.003; Nadult = 12, z = −3.1, p = 0.002), whereas no significant difference was found in juvenile individuals (Njuvenile = 12, z = −0.775, p = 0.439)

In terms of reward quantity, subadult and adult subjects waited to be proposed more food rewards before returning the token when it was a higher-valued token (i.e. a high-valued rather than a medium-valued token; Wilcoxon tests, Nsubadult = 12, z = −3.1, p = 0.002; Nadult = 40, z = −3.0, p = 0.003)

We also found that the percentage of successful token/reward-bartering sequences when more than one of the proposed food rewards were accumulated by the monkey before returning the token was significantly higher when the monkey was holding a high-valued token than when it was holding a medium-valued token, both in subadult (Wilcoxon test, N = 10, z = −2.8, p = 0.005) and adult subjects (N = 29, z = −2.4, p = 0.015)

In terms of reward quality, and as expected, the average percentage of rejections, by adult subjects, of the least-preferred type of food rewards (when at least one reward among the least-preferred type was proposed during a given token/reward-bartering sequence) was significantly higher when they were holding a high-valued token, compared to a medium-valued token (Wilcoxon test, N = 22, z = −2.3, p = 0.024)

Contrary to our expectations, there was no significant difference in the average number of food rewards being rejected by the monkey before accepting a different type of food reward to end the token/reward-bartering sequence when it was holding a high-valued token, compared to a medium-valued token, either in subadults (Wilcoxon test, N = 9, z = −1.2, p = 0.223) or in adults (Wilcoxon test, N = 29, z = −1.1, p = 0.280)

Subadult and adult subjects were significantly more likely to end a successful token/reward-bartering sequence by returning the token in exchange for the least-preferred type of food rewards (provided at least one reward among the least-preferred type was proposed during a given token/reward-bartering sequence) when they were holding a medium-valued token compared to when they were holding a high-valued token (Wilcoxon tests, Nsubadult = 10, z = −2.2, p = 0.028; Nadult = 22, z = −2.1, p = 0.033)

We found a significant increase in token-robbing success from juveniles to subadults to adults, whereas the main behaviour patterns required for the successful performance of token/reward-bartering interactions were already in place from around 4 years (i.e. in subadults)

Discussion

This field observational and experimental study of token-robbing and token/reward-bartering interactions in the free-ranging population of Balinese long-tailed macaques produced three main findings: (i) these behaviours need to be learned throughout juvenescence (i.e. until up to 4 years in this species) to be successfully performed; (ii) older monkeys preferentially selected tokens that were more valued by humans; and (iii) these more skilful and selective individuals appeared to make economic decisions, as evidenced by clear behavioural associations between value-based token possession and quantity or quality of food rewards rejected and accepted.

As predicted, we found a significant increase in token-robbing success from juveniles to subadults to adults, whereas the main behaviour patterns required for the successful performance of token/reward-bartering interactions were already in place from around 4 years (i.e. in subadults). Likewise, the ability to engage in more negotiated successful token/reward-bartering sequences—during which the monkey only returned the token after being proposed more food rewards, or after rejecting more food rewards, or after accepting a type of food reward different from the one(s) previously rejected—was not fully acquired before the subadult stage.

These results lend some support to the ‘experiential learning&#x27; hypothesis, whereby token-robbing and token/reward-bartering interactions are multi-stepped and complex behavioural sequences requiring perceptual learning, sensorimotor coordination and cognitive skills (e.g. memory, associative learning) to be successfully performed; they are thus gradually acquired through extended individual practice during the juvenile period, in part via experiential trial-and-error learning. It is noteworthy to mention that the development of (sub)adult-level proficiency at robbing/bartering is not only dependent on skill learning (e.g. detection, sneaky approach, self-control), but may also be constrained by physical maturation. This is particularly true during the token-robbing phase that often involves monkey–human body contact and/or requires muscular strength when a monkey has to yank on a flip-flop still worn by an adult human. In these cases, the limited physical capabilities of juveniles, and the maturing bodies of subadults, may partly explain the significant increase in token-robbing success from juveniles to subadults to adults.

Primates are characterized by the longest juvenile period in relation to life span of all mammals [20]. According to the ‘needing-to-learn' hypothesis [20], prolonged juvenility is associated with behavioural patterns that necessitate acquiring a proportionally large amount of information and/or skills to reach adult competence before individuals become reproductively mature. These behaviours include extractive foraging techniques [21] and (socio-)sexual behaviour patterns [22]. Our study indicates that both phases of the robbing/bartering practice also required experiential learning to be fully mastered.

The first step in economic decision-making requires the cognitive ability to distinguish among different expected material values of a given symbolic currency (e.g. tokens, cash, virtual money). After showing that token selection was not significantly affected by token availability and the relative ease with which different types of tokens were stolen by the monkeys from human targets, our observational data revealed a marked age difference in how the monkeys responded to a human-based three-level hierarchy of valuable objects. When considering the token selection among all the prospective human targets (i.e. temple visitors with potential tokens available in a given area), juveniles did not show any preferential selection among low-valued, medium-valued and high-valued tokens, whereas subadults and adults preferentially selected high- and medium-valued tokens over low-valued ones. When two tokens of different values were available on a given human target, subadults and adults preferentially selected the higher-value token, whereas juvenile individuals did not show any significant difference. We found a similar age difference in the value-based token selection, after experimentally controlling for token accessibility and lateral bias.

These results support the ‘value-based token selection' hypothesis, positing age differences in the selection of higher-valued tokens by the monkeys during the token-robbing phase that are indicative of a developmental trajectory toward more strategic choices in more mature individuals. Subadult and adult monkeys (but not juveniles yet) have learned to map their token-robbing behaviours onto the hierarchical (and arbitrary) scale of values attributed by humans to different tokens: they preferentially selected tokens that were more likely to be exchanged for food (e.g. electronic devices, pairs of glasses) over other objects that were less valuable for humans and typically not worth bartering (e.g. empty camera bags, hairpins). Our findings are consistent with data obtained in other non-human primate species, showing that subadult and adult capuchin monkeys and chimpanzees correctly preferred a high-valued token over a low-valued token in an experimental bartering situation [1,11,23].

The second step in economic decision-making requires the cognitive ability (i.e. mental processes involving associative learning and memorization) to respond differently to differentially valued tokens by trying to maximize one's payoff. We found evidence for such behavioural associations between value-based token possession and quantity or quality of food rewards rejected and accepted by subadult and adult monkeys (i.e. the most skilful and selective individuals) during the token/reward-bartering phase. They consistently and actively obtained either more food rewards or a more preferred food reward in exchange for a higher-valued token. They were also more likely to end a successful bartering interaction by accepting a less preferred food reward in exchange for a lower-valued token.

Our findings support the ‘robbing/bartering payoff maximization' hypothesis in that subadult and adult monkeys strategically responded to differentially valued tokens in their possession by adjusting the amount or type of food rewards they gained from the barters. The result showing that subadults (unlike adults) failed to significantly reject more low-preferred food rewards before returning a higher-valued token may be explained in terms of poorer temporal cognition or higher impulsivity (compared to adults): subadult long-tailed macaques may either not have yet acquired the cognitive capacity to anticipate the subsequent proffering of more preferred food rewards in this specific situation, or not be patient/self-controlled enough to wait for possibly more preferred food rewards.

Overall, our field observational data are in line with laboratory-based studies showing that several non-human primate species can (i) understand the effectiveness of tokens as secondary reinforcements to make simple calculations about quantities of reward, (ii) determine an item's value on the basis of its perceived utility (e.g. exchanging only a low-preferred reward for a tool necessary to reach a more preferred reward) and (iii) recognize the appropriate conditions in which a successful exchange could occur (e.g. presence/absence of the experimenter, safe/risky experimenter) [1,2,23,24,25,26,27]. Other cognitive skills and temperamental traits exhibited to varying extents by non-human primates engaging in token-aided economic behaviours include preference transitivity, self-control, delay of gratification, action planning and calculated reciprocity, because they may facilitate or constrain an individual's ability to make optimal economic decisions [1,5,23,28]. Even though these characteristics were not explicitly examined in this study, some of them will be the subject of our future observational and experimental investigations.

Conclusion

Token-robbing and token/reward-bartering are cognitively challenging tasks for the Uluwatu macaques that revealed unprecedented economic decision-making processes (i.e. valued-based token selection and payoff maximization) in a large monkey population living in an anthropogenically impacted habitat. This spontaneous, population-specific, prevalent, cross-generational, learned and socially influenced practice may be the first example of a culturally maintained token economy in free-ranging animals. The present naturalistic research setting represents a unique opportunity to study field economics and explore macroeconomic phenomena in non-human primates in environmental conditions that are more externally and ecologically valid than those provided by the traditional token exchange paradigm. Further experimental research on the Uluwatu macaques should make future cross-species comparisons of economic decision-making and symbolic tool use more relevant from an evolutionary perspective and may ultimately lead to a better understanding of the origins of autonomous monetary systems in humans [29].

The research protocol of this study was approved by the Animal Welfare Committee of The University of Lethbridge (Alberta, Canada) (Protocol #1430). Our study was conducted with research permission from the Indonesian Ministry of Research and Technology (#328/SIP/FRP/E5/Dit.KI/IX/2015; #410/SIP/FRP/E5/Dit.KI/X/2015), related Indonesian government and provincial agencies and the local authorities of the Uluwatu Temple.

Study compliance

Ethics

The research protocol of this study was approved by the Animal Welfare Committee of The University of Lethbridge (Alberta, Canada) (Protocol #1430)

We declare we have no competing interests.

Funding

For this study, J.-B.L. was funded by a Discovery Grant from the Natural Sciences and Engineering Research Council of Canada (2015-06034), a Major Grant from the Alberta Gambling Research Institute (43699-4200-8001), a research allowance from the Office of the Dean of Arts and Science at the University of Lethbridge (14460-4200-8015) and a University of Lethbridge Research Fund (13424-4200-8015).

Data and code

The dataset ‘Acquisition of object-robbing and object/food-bartering behaviours: a culturally maintained token economy in free-ranging long-tailed macaques’ is available from the Dryad Digital Repository: https://doi.org/10.5061/dryad.wpzgmsbjh [30]. Authors' contributions J.-B.L.contributed to research design, data analysis and manuscript drafting. N.G.conducted video data scoring and contributed to research design, data analysis and manuscript drafting. M.G.contributed to research design, data collection, data analysis and manuscript drafting. I.N.W.assisted with research permission and contributed to research design. All authors gave final approval for publication and agreed to be held accountable for the work performed therein.

Spotlight
Important contexts

1. Introduction

The token exchange paradigm is an appealing and heuristically powerful system used to investigate the existence of economic behaviour in non-human primates and to explore the evolutionary origins and developmental pathways of human monetary systems.

1. Introduction

Like a symbolic tool, a token is an inherently non-valuable (e.g. inedible or non-nutritive) object that acts as a secondary conditioned reinforcer; indeed, it only acquires an instrumental or functional value through the arbitrary associations made with the goods (e.g. food) or services (e.g. social or sexual favours) it is conventionally exchanged for in a barter-like situation, even though the object bears no iconic relation to its referent [1,2]. The tokens used by primate subjects in such experimental studies physically resemble human coins, and the act of trading tokens is similar to that of exchanging money for other commodities. The token exchange paradigm has shown that several species of monkeys and great apes can use tokens to request specific food rewards.

1. Introduction

Around the Uluwatu Temple in Bali, Indonesia, a large free-ranging population of long-tailed macaques (Macaca fascicularis) spontaneously and routinely engage in token-mediated bartering interactions with humans. These interactions occur in two phases: after stealing inedible and more or less valuable objects (e.g. pairs of glasses, hats, empty bags) from temple visitors, the macaques appear to use them as tokens, by returning them to humans in exchange for a certain number/type of food rewards proffered by the temple staff [10].

1. Introduction

In many respects, these naturally occurring token-robbing and token/reward-bartering interactions are reminiscent of the token exchange paradigm experimentally implemented by researchers in captive settings, in which the symbolic value of a token lies in the quantity and quality of the food reward gained in return [2,8,9,10,11,12,13,14,15].

1. Introduction

However, we do not know whether this behaviour is learned through individual practice (i.e. via experiential trial-and-error learning) and underlain by economic decision-making processes—that is, subjected to robbing/bartering payoff maximization as would be suggested by clear behavioural associations between value-based token possession and quantity or quality of food rewards rejected and accepted by the monkeys.

1. Introduction

From juveniles to subadults to adults, we predicted an increase in the relative selection of higher-valued tokens available among all the temple visitors present on the site (Prediction 2a). When two tokens of different values were available on a given temple visitor, we predicted that subadult and adult monkeys would preferentially select the higher-valued token, whereas no significant difference was expected in juvenile monkeys (Prediction 2b).

2. Methods

Observational data were based on the spontaneous expression of token-robbing and token/reward-bartering interactions between monkeys and humans, with no intervention from the researchers.

2. Methods

These data were collected by using the combined ‘behaviour-dependent sampling' rule and the ‘continuous recording' rule [18]. Behaviour-dependent sampling typically applies to data collection on ‘conspicuous' or ‘attention-attracting' behaviours [18, p.87]; these are characteristics that fit token-robbing and token/reward-bartering interactions, particularly because they usually occur within a few wide and open areas located around Uluwatu temple, where visitors are allowed and visibility for the researchers was good [10,16].

2. Methods

However, this data collection method allowed us to quantify all the other variables used in this study (i.e. individual percentages of robbing/bartering successful attempts; number and type of object(s) being available on, and selected from, a human target; individual average numbers of food rewards being proposed, rejected, and accepted) because these measures were based on the relative behavioural frequencies calculated from a total of recorded events.

2. Methods

We used individual percentages of successful token-robbing attempts and successful token/reward-bartering attempts as valid measures of experiential learning by conducting cross-sectional analyses based on age differences.

2. Methods

To assess the relative local availability in the six different types of tokens, we examined a sample of 84 video-recorded token-robbing events, randomly selected from our observational data, that featured 500 potential human targets.

2. Methods

For analyses on food rewards rejected and accepted by the monkeys during the token/reward-bartering phase, we only considered the food items that the monkey could reach/grab when proposed/tossed to them by the human barterers.

2. Methods

To test whether the monkeys selected the six different types of tokens based on their relative local availability, we used a Spearman rank-order correlation test between the numbers of the six types of tokens available from our sample of 500 potential human targets and the numbers of the six types of tokens selected by the monkeys within our entire set of observational data (i.e. the sum of Nmissed and Ntaken for each type of token).

3. Results

The percentages of successful token-robbing attempts were 39.1% (N = 376 attempts), 61.5% (N = 1829 attempts) and 68.8% (N = 3062 attempts) in juveniles, subadults, and adults, respectively. We found a statistically significant age difference in the percentage of successful token-robbing attempts (Kruskal–Wallis test, H2 = 35.8, p < 0.001).

3. Results

Post hoc pairwise comparisons revealed that subadults were more successful than juveniles (Mann–Whitney test, Nsubadult = 20, Njuvenile = 17, U = 69.0, p = 0.002), adults were more successful than juveniles (Nadult = 60, U = 68.5, p < 0.001), and adults were more successful than subadults (U = 302.5, p = 0.001; figure 1).

3. Results

Therefore, we found a significant increase in token-robbing success from juveniles to subadults to adults; Prediction 1a was supported.

3. Results

The percentages of successful token/reward-bartering attempts were 72.4% (N = 58 attempts), 89.4% (N = 689 attempts) and 92.0% (N = 1446 attempts) in juveniles, subadults and adults, respectively. We found a statistically significant age difference in the percentage of successful token/reward-bartering attempts (H2 = 22.3, p < 0.001).

3. Results

Post hoc pairwise comparisons revealed that subadults were more successful than juveniles (Nsubadult = 13, Njuvenile = 10, U = 9.5, p = 0.001) and adults were more successful than juveniles (Nadult = 45, U = 20.0, p < 0.001).

3. Results

However, adults and subadults did not significantly differ in percentages of successful token/reward-bartering attempts (U = 213.0, p = 0.135; figure 1). Even though we did not find a significant increase in token-robbing success from juveniles to subadults to adults, Prediction 1b was partly supported owing to the difference between the former and the latter two age classes.

3. Results

During successful token/reward-bartering sequences, we found statistically significant age differences in the average number of food rewards being proposed to the monkey (H2 = 15.1, p = 0.001), the average number of food rewards being rejected by the monkey (H2 = 13.9, p = 0.001) and the average number of food rewards being rejected by the monkey before accepting a different type of food reward to end the token/reward-bartering sequence (H2 = 8.7, p = 0.013).

3. Results

However, adults and subadults did not significantly differ in these scores (U = 301.0, p = 0.713; U = 313.5, p = 0.882; U = 305.0, p = 0.766, respectively). Even though we did not find a significant increase in the scores indicating negotiated successful token/reward-bartering sequences from juveniles to subadults to adults, Prediction 1c was partly supported owing to the difference between the former and the latter two age classes.

3. Results

In the observational data, the relative local availability in the six types of tokens and the numbers of the six types of tokens selected by the monkeys within our entire set of observational data were not statistically correlated (Spearman's correlation coefficient, N = 6, Rs = −0.17, p = 0.742).

3. Results

Moreover, the average percentage of successful token-robbing attempts among all the subadult and adult monkeys (i.e. the most skilled robbers) and the percentage of types of tokens selected from human targets were not statistically correlated (Spearman's correlation coefficient, N = 6, Rs = −0.26, p = 0.623).

3. Results

However, we found statistically significant differences in the selection of the three differentially valued tokens by subadults (N = 20, χ2 = 8.5, p = 0.014) and adults (N = 60, χ2 = 21.4, p < 0.001).

3. Results

Post hoc pairwise comparisons revealed that subadults and adults preferentially selected high-valued over low-valued tokens (Wilcoxon tests, subadults: z = −2.0, p = 0.049; adults: z = −3.6, p < 0.001) and medium-valued over low-valued tokens (subadults: z = −2.0, p = 0.049; adults: z = −2.6, p = 0.010). There was no significant difference in the selection of high-valued or medium-valued tokens in subadults (z = −0.2, p = 0.872) and adults (z = −0.1, p = 0.997). Even though we did not find a significant increase in the relative selection of higher-valued tokens among all the prospective human targets from juveniles to subadults to adults, Prediction 2a was partly supported owing to the difference between the former and the latter two age classes.

3. Results

However, when two tokens of different values were available on a given human target, we found that subadult and adult monkeys preferentially selected the higher-valued token (Wilcoxon tests, Nsubadult = 12, z = −3.0, p = 0.003; Nadult = 12, z = −3.1, p = 0.002), whereas no significant difference was found in juvenile individuals (Njuvenile = 12, z = −0.775, p = 0.439).

3. Results

In the experimental data, when two tokens of different values were available on plastic trays placed in front of a tested monkey, we found that subadult and adult subjects preferentially selected the higher-valued token (Binomial tests, subadults: Nmedium-valued tokens selected = 12, Nhigh-valued tokens selected = 68, p < 0.001; adults: Nmedium-valued tokens selected = 10, Nhigh-valued tokens selected = 70, p < 0.001), whereas no significant difference was found in juvenile subjects (Nmedium-valued tokens selected = 35, Nhigh-valued tokens selected = 45, p = 0.314).

3. Results

In this section about economic decision-making (i.e. testing behavioural associations between value-based token possession and quantity or quality of food rewards rejected and accepted), we focused on the most skilful and selective individuals during the token/reward-bartering phase, namely, subadult and adult monkeys.

3. Results

In terms of reward quantity, subadult and adult subjects waited to be proposed more food rewards before returning the token when it was a higher-valued token (i.e. a high-valued rather than a medium-valued token; Wilcoxon tests, Nsubadult = 12, z = −3.1, p = 0.002; Nadult = 40, z = −3.0, p = 0.003). Low-valued tokens could not be used in these analyses because, by definition, they were typically not bartered by the temple staff.

3. Results

We also found that the percentage of successful token/reward-bartering sequences when more than one of the proposed food rewards were accumulated by the monkey before returning the token was significantly higher when the monkey was holding a high-valued token than when it was holding a medium-valued token, both in subadult (Wilcoxon test, N = 10, z = −2.8, p = 0.005) and adult subjects (N = 29, z = −2.4, p = 0.015).

3. Results

Therefore, Prediction 3c was supported for adult, but not for subadult subjects.

3. Results

Contrary to our expectations, there was no significant difference in the average number of food rewards being rejected by the monkey before accepting a different type of food reward to end the token/reward-bartering sequence when it was holding a high-valued token, compared to a medium-valued token, either in subadults (Wilcoxon test, N = 9, z = −1.2, p = 0.223) or in adults (Wilcoxon test, N = 29, z = −1.1, p = 0.280).

4. Discussion

This field observational and experimental study of token-robbing and token/reward-bartering interactions in the free-ranging population of Balinese long-tailed macaques produced three main findings: (i) these behaviours need to be learned throughout juvenescence (i.e. until up to 4 years in this species) to be successfully performed; (ii) older monkeys preferentially selected tokens that were more valued by humans; and (iii) these more skilful and selective individuals appeared to make economic decisions, as evidenced by clear behavioural associations between value-based token possession and quantity or quality of food rewards rejected and accepted.

4. Discussion

As predicted, we found a significant increase in token-robbing success from juveniles to subadults to adults, whereas the main behaviour patterns required for the successful performance of token/reward-bartering interactions were already in place from around 4 years (i.e. in subadults).

4. Discussion

Likewise, the ability to engage in more negotiated successful token/reward-bartering sequences—during which the monkey only returned the token after being proposed more food rewards, or after rejecting more food rewards, or after accepting a type of food reward different from the one(s) previously rejected—was not fully acquired before the subadult stage.

4. Discussion

These results lend some support to the ‘experiential learning&#x27; hypothesis, whereby token-robbing and token/reward-bartering interactions are multi-stepped and complex behavioural sequences requiring perceptual learning, sensorimotor coordination and cognitive skills (e.g. memory, associative learning) to be successfully performed; they are thus gradually acquired through extended individual practice during the juvenile period, in part via experiential trial-and-error learning.

4. Discussion

This is particularly true during the token-robbing phase that often involves monkey–human body contact and/or requires muscular strength when a monkey has to yank on a flip-flop still worn by an adult human.

4. Discussion

Primates are characterized by the longest juvenile period in relation to life span of all mammals [20].

4. Discussion

According to the ‘needing-to-learn' hypothesis [20], prolonged juvenility is associated with behavioural patterns that necessitate acquiring a proportionally large amount of information and/or skills to reach adult competence before individuals become reproductively mature.

4. Discussion

These behaviours include extractive foraging techniques [21] and (socio-)sexual behaviour patterns [22]. Our study indicates that both phases of the robbing/bartering practice also required experiential learning to be fully mastered.

4. Discussion

After showing that token selection was not significantly affected by token availability and the relative ease with which different types of tokens were stolen by the monkeys from human targets, our observational data revealed a marked age difference in how the monkeys responded to a human-based three-level hierarchy of valuable objects.

4. Discussion

When considering the token selection among all the prospective human targets (i.e. temple visitors with potential tokens available in a given area), juveniles did not show any preferential selection among low-valued, medium-valued and high-valued tokens, whereas subadults and adults preferentially selected high- and medium-valued tokens over low-valued ones. When two tokens of different values were available on a given human target, subadults and adults preferentially selected the higher-value token, whereas juvenile individuals did not show any significant difference. We found a similar age difference in the value-based token selection, after experimentally controlling for token accessibility and lateral bias.

4. Discussion

These results support the ‘value-based token selection' hypothesis, positing age differences in the selection of higher-valued tokens by the monkeys during the token-robbing phase that are indicative of a developmental trajectory toward more strategic choices in more mature individuals.

4. Discussion

Subadult and adult monkeys (but not juveniles yet) have learned to map their token-robbing behaviours onto the hierarchical (and arbitrary) scale of values attributed by humans to different tokens: they preferentially selected tokens that were more likely to be exchanged for food (e.g. electronic devices, pairs of glasses) over other objects that were less valuable for humans and typically not worth bartering (e.g. empty camera bags, hairpins). Our findings are consistent with data obtained in other non-human primate species, showing that subadult and adult capuchin monkeys and chimpanzees correctly preferred a high-valued token over a low-valued token in an experimental bartering situation [1,11,23].

4. Discussion

The second step in economic decision-making requires the cognitive ability (i.e. mental processes involving associative learning and memorization) to respond differently to differentially valued tokens by trying to maximize one's payoff. We found evidence for such behavioural associations between value-based token possession and quantity or quality of food rewards rejected and accepted by subadult and adult monkeys (i.e. the most skilful and selective individuals) during the token/reward-bartering phase.

4. Discussion

Our findings support the ‘robbing/bartering payoff maximization' hypothesis in that subadult and adult monkeys strategically responded to differentially valued tokens in their possession by adjusting the amount or type of food rewards they gained from the barters.

4. Discussion

The result showing that subadults (unlike adults) failed to significantly reject more low-preferred food rewards before returning a higher-valued token may be explained in terms of poorer temporal cognition or higher impulsivity (compared to adults): subadult long-tailed macaques may either not have yet acquired the cognitive capacity to anticipate the subsequent proffering of more preferred food rewards in this specific situation, or not be patient/self-controlled enough to wait for possibly more preferred food rewards.

4. Discussion

Other cognitive skills and temperamental traits exhibited to varying extents by non-human primates engaging in token-aided economic behaviours include preference transitivity, self-control, delay of gratification, action planning and calculated reciprocity, because they may facilitate or constrain an individual's ability to make optimal economic decisions [1,5,23,28].

5. Conclusion

The present naturalistic research setting represents a unique opportunity to study field economics and explore macroeconomic phenomena in non-human primates in environmental conditions that are more externally and ecologically valid than those provided by the traditional token exchange paradigm.

5. Conclusion

Further experimental research on the Uluwatu macaques should make future cross-species comparisons of economic decision-making and symbolic tool use more relevant from an evolutionary perspective and may ultimately lead to a better understanding of the origins of autonomous monetary systems in humans [29].

5. Conclusion

The research protocol of this study was approved by the Animal Welfare Committee of The University of Lethbridge (Alberta, Canada) (Protocol #1430). Our study was conducted with research permission from the Indonesian Ministry of Research and Technology (#328/SIP/FRP/E5/Dit.KI/IX/2015; #410/SIP/FRP/E5/Dit.KI/X/2015), related Indonesian government and provincial agencies and the local authorities of the Uluwatu Temple.

Contributions

To assess the relative local availability in the six different types of tokens, we examined a sample of 84 video-recorded token-robbing events, randomly selected from our observational data, that featured 500 potential human targets

For analyses on food rewards rejected and accepted by the monkeys during the token/reward-bartering phase, we only considered the food items that the monkey could reach/grab when proposed/tossed to them by the human barterers

We found a statistically significant age difference in the percentage of successful token-robbing attempts (Kruskal–Wallis test, H2 = 35.8, p &lt; 0.001)

We found a significant increase in token-robbing success from juveniles to subadults to adults; Prediction 1a was supported

We found a statistically significant age difference in the percentage of successful token/reward-bartering attempts (H2 = 22.3, p &lt; 0.001)

Even though we did not find a significant increase in token-robbing success from juveniles to subadults to adults, Prediction 1b was partly supported owing to the difference between the former and the latter two age classes

During successful token/reward-bartering sequences, we found statistically significant age differences in the average number of food rewards being proposed to the monkey (H2 = 15.1, p = 0.001), the average number of food rewards being rejected by the monkey (H2 = 13.9, p = 0.001) and the average number of food rewards being rejected by the monkey before accepting a different type of food reward to end the token/reward-bartering sequence (H2 = 8.7, p = 0.013)

Even though we did not find a significant increase in the scores indicating negotiated successful token/reward-bartering sequences from juveniles to subadults to adults, Prediction 1c was partly supported owing to the difference between the former and the latter two age classes. (b) ‘Value-based token selection&#x27; hypothesis

We found statistically significant differences in the selection of the three differentially valued tokens by subadults (N = 20, χ2 = 8.5, p = 0.014) and adults (N = 60, χ2 = 21.4, p &lt; 0.001)

Even though we did not find a significant increase in the relative selection of higher-valued tokens among all the prospective human targets from juveniles to subadults to adults, Prediction 2a was partly supported owing to the difference between the former and the latter two age classes

When two tokens of different values were available on a given human target, we found that subadult and adult monkeys preferentially selected the higher-valued token (Wilcoxon tests, Nsubadult = 12, z = −3.0, p = 0.003; Nadult = 12, z = −3.1, p = 0.002), whereas no significant difference was found in juvenile individuals (Njuvenile = 12, z = −0.775, p = 0.439)

In the experimental data, when two tokens of different values were available on plastic trays placed in front of a tested monkey, we found that subadult and adult subjects preferentially selected the higher-valued token (Binomial tests, subadults: Nmedium-valued tokens selected = 12, Nhigh-valued tokens selected = 68, p &lt; 0.001; adults: Nmedium-valued tokens selected = 10, Nhigh-valued tokens selected = 70, p &lt; 0.001), whereas no significant difference was found in juvenile subjects (Nmedium-valued tokens selected = 35, Nhigh-valued tokens selected = 45, p = 0.314)

About economic decision-making (i.e. testing behavioural associations between value-based token possession and quantity or quality of food rewards rejected and accepted), we focused on the most skilful and selective individuals during the token/reward-bartering phase, namely, subadult and adult monkeys

We also found that the percentage of successful token/reward-bartering sequences when more than one of the proposed food rewards were accumulated by the monkey before returning the token was significantly higher when the monkey was holding a high-valued token than when it was holding a medium-valued token, both in subadult (Wilcoxon test, N = 10, z = −2.8, p = 0.005) and adult subjects (N = 29, z = −2.4, p = 0.015)

We found a significant increase in token-robbing success from juveniles to subadults to adults, whereas the main behaviour patterns required for the successful performance of token/reward-bartering interactions were already in place from around 4 years (i.e. in subadults)

Our study indicates that both phases of the robbing/bartering practice also required experiential learning to be fully mastered

We found a similar age difference in the value-based token selection, after experimentally controlling for token accessibility and lateral bias

We found evidence for such behavioural associations between value-based token possession and quantity or quality of food rewards rejected and accepted by subadult and adult monkeys (i.e. the most skilful and selective individuals) during the token/reward-bartering phase

Important points

The token exchange paradigm shows that monkeys

These interactions occur in two phases

the macaques appear to use them as tokens

heuristically powerful system used to investigate the existence of economic behaviour in non-human primates

functional value through the arbitrary associations made with the goods or services

even though the object bears no iconic relation to its referent

The tokens used by primate subjects in such experimental studies physically resemble human coins

The token exchange paradigm has shown that several species of monkeys

pairwise comparisons revealed that subadults were more successful than juveniles

adults were more successful than juveniles

adults were more successful than subadults

we found a significant increase in token-robbing success from juveniles

Even though we did not find a significant increase in token-robbing success from juveniles

Prediction 1b was partly supported owing to the difference

statistically significant age differences in the average number of food rewards being proposed to the monkey

the average number of food rewards being rejected by the monkey

the average number of food rewards being rejected by the monkey before accepting a different type

subadults scored significantly higher than juveniles and adults scored higher than juveniles

Even though we did not find a significant increase in the scores indicating negotiated successful token

Prediction 1c was partly supported owing to the difference

the numbers of the six types of tokens selected by the monkeys within our entire set

the percentage of types of tokens selected from human targets were

pairwise comparisons revealed that subadults and adults preferentially selected high-valued over low-valued

There was no significant difference in the selection of high-valued

Even though we did not find a significant increase in the relative selection of higher-valued tokens

Prediction 2a was partly supported owing to the difference

when two tokens of different values were available on a given human target

whereas no significant difference was found in juvenile individuals

when two tokens of different values were available on plastic trays placed in front

whereas no significant difference was found in juvenile subjects

Low-valued tokens could not be used in these analyses

they were typically not bartered by the temple staff

We found that the percentage of successful token

of the proposed food rewards were accumulated by the monkey before returning the token was significantly higher when the monkey was holding a high-valued token

Prediction 3c was supported for adult

there was no significant difference in the average number of food rewards being rejected by the monkey before accepting a different type

these behaviours need to be learned throughout juvenescence to be successfully performed

older monkeys preferentially selected tokens that were more valued by humans

whereas the main behaviour patterns required for the successful performance of token

the ability to engage in more negotiated successful token

These results lend some support to the ‘experiential learning&#x27

they are gradually acquired through extended individual practice during the juvenile period

noteworthy to mention that the development ofadult-level proficiency

bartering is dependent on skill learning

This is true during the token-robbing phase

requires muscular strength when a monkey has to yank on a flip-flop still worn by an adult human

Primates are characterized by the longest juvenile period in relation

prolonged juvenility is associated with behavioural patterns

Our study indicates that both phases of the robbing

the relative ease with which different types of tokens were stolen by the monkeys from human targets

how the monkeys responded to a human-based three-level hierarchy of valuable objects

When two tokens of different values were available on a given human target

positing age differences in the selection of higher-valued tokens by the monkeys during the token-robbing phase that are indicative of a developmental trajectory toward more strategic choices

map their token-robbing behaviours onto the hierarchical scale of values attributed by humans to different tokens

likely to be exchanged for food over other objects

that were less valuable for humans

worth bartering. Our findings are consistent with data obtained in other

adult capuchin monkeys and chimpanzees correctly preferred a high-valued token over a low-valued token in an experimental bartering situation

adult monkeys strategically responded to differentially valued tokens in their possession

by adjusting the amount or type of food rewards they gained from the barters

The result showing that subadults failed

a higher-valued token may be explained in terms of poorer temporal cognition

yet acquired the cognitive capacity to anticipate the subsequent proffering of more preferred food rewards in this specific situation

our field observational data are in line with laboratory-based studies showing that several non-human primate species can understand the effectiveness of

temperamental traits exhibited to varying extents by non-human primates engaging in token-aided economic behaviours include

valid than those provided by the traditional token exchange paradigm

symbolic tool use more relevant from an evolutionary perspective

The research protocol of this study was approved by the Animal Welfare Committee of The University

Our study was conducted with research permission from the Indonesian Ministry

type of food rewards proffered by the temple staff

reward-bartering interactions are reminiscent of the token exchange paradigm experimentally implemented by researchers

in which the symbolic value of a token lies in the quantity and quality of the food reward gained in return

We do not know whether this behaviour is learned through individual practice

we predicted an increase in token-robbing success

When two tokens of different values were available on a given temple visitor

whereas no significant difference was expected in juvenile monkeys

Observational data were based on the spontaneous expression of token-robbing

Behaviour-dependent sampling typically applies to data collection

open areas located around Uluwatu temple

because these measures were based on the relative behavioural frequencies calculated from a total

reward-bartering attempts as valid measures of experiential learning by conducting cross-sectional analyses based on age differences

we examined a sample of 84 video-recorded token-robbing events

We found a significant increase in token-robbing success from juveniles

Our findings are consistent with data obtained in other

Our field observational data are in line with laboratory-based studies showing that several non-human primate species can understand the effectiveness of

Key concepts

economic behaviour economic behaviour economic behaviour

token economy token economy token economy token economy token economy

economy economy economy economy economy

Macaca fascicularis

Abstract
The token exchange paradigm shows that monkeys and great apes are able to use objects as symbolic tools to request specific food rewards. Such studies provide insights into the cognitive underpinnings of economic behaviour in non-human primates. However, the ecological validity of these laboratory-based experimental situations tends to be limited. Our field research aims to address the need for a more ecologically valid primate model of trading systems in humans. Around the Uluwatu Temple in Bali, Indonesia, a large free-ranging population of long-tailed macaques spontaneously and routinely engage in token-mediated bartering interactions with humans. These interactions occur in two phases: after stealing inedible and more or less valuable objects from humans, the macaques appear to use them as tokens, by returning them to humans in exchange for food. Our field observational and experimental data showed (i) age differences in robbing/bartering success, indicative of experiential learning, and (ii) clear behavioural associations between value-based token possession and quantity or quality of food rewards rejected and accepted by subadult and adult monkeys, suggestive of robbing/bartering payoff maximization and economic decision-making. This population-specific, prevalent, cross-generational, learned and socially influenced practice may be the first example of a culturally maintained token economy in free-ranging animals. This article is part of the theme issue ‘Existence and prevalence of economic behaviours among non-human primates'.
Full text

Front matter

Your password must have 8 characters or more and contain 3 of the following:

Enter your email address below and we will send you the reset instructions

If the address matches an existing account you will receive an email with instructions to reset your password.

Enter your email address below and we will send you your username

If the address matches an existing account you will receive an email with instructions to retrieve your username

Skip main navigationJournal menuClose Drawer MenuOpen Drawer Menu

Notes and Records

Enter words, phrases, DOI, keywords, authors, etc...

Leca Jean-Baptiste, Gunst Noëlle, Gardiner Matthew and Wandia I. Nengah

2021Acquisition of object-robbing and object/food-bartering behaviours: a culturally maintained token economy in free-ranging long-tailed macaquesPhil. Trans. R. Soc. B37620190677http://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2019.0677

Acquisition of object-robbing and object/food-bartering behaviours: a culturally maintained token economy in free-ranging long-tailed macaques

Department of Psychology, University of Lethbridge, Lethbridge, Alberta, Canada T1K 3M4

I. Nengah Wandia

Faculty of Veterinary Medicine, Udayana University, Bukit Jimbaran, Bali, Indonesia

I. Nengah Wandia

I. Nengah Wandia

Published:11 January 2021https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2019.0677

Main text

The token exchange paradigm shows that monkeys and great apes are able to use objects as symbolic tools to request specific food rewards. Such studies provide insights into the cognitive underpinnings of economic behaviour in non-human primates. However, the ecological validity of these laboratory-based experimental situations tends to be limited. Our field research aims to address the need for a more ecologically valid primate model of trading systems in humans. Around the Uluwatu Temple in Bali, Indonesia, a large free-ranging population of long-tailed macaques spontaneously and routinely engage in token-mediated bartering interactions with humans. These interactions occur in two phases: after stealing inedible and more or less valuable objects from humans, the macaques appear to use them as tokens, by returning them to humans in exchange for food. Our field observational and experimental data showed (i) age differences in robbing/bartering success, indicative of experiential learning, and (ii) clear behavioural associations between value-based token possession and quantity or quality of food rewards rejected and accepted by subadult and adult monkeys, suggestive of robbing/bartering payoff maximization and economic decision-making. This population-specific, prevalent, cross-generational, learned and socially influenced practice may be the first example of a culturally maintained token economy in free-ranging animals.

This article is part of the theme issue ‘Existence and prevalence of economic behaviours among non-human primates'.

1. Introduction

The token exchange paradigm is an appealing and heuristically powerful system used to investigate the existence of economic behaviour in non-human primates and to explore the evolutionary origins and developmental pathways of human monetary systems. Like a symbolic tool, a token is an inherently non-valuable (e.g. inedible or non-nutritive) object that acts as a secondary conditioned reinforcer; indeed, it only acquires an instrumental or functional value through the arbitrary associations made with the goods (e.g. food) or services (e.g. social or sexual favours) it is conventionally exchanged for in a barter-like situation, even though the object bears no iconic relation to its referent [1,2]. The tokens used by primate subjects in such experimental studies physically resemble human coins, and the act of trading tokens is similar to that of exchanging money for other commodities. The token exchange paradigm has shown that several species of monkeys and great apes can use tokens to request specific food rewards. This line of research provides insights into the cognitive underpinnings of economic behaviour in non-human primates [3].

However, most of these experimental procedures involve human-induced exchanges with relatively small samples of individually trained, laboratory-bred subjects. During the experiments, these subjects (i) are typically placed in isolation from their conspecifics and their other daily activities, (ii) exchanged in constrained environments characterized by a lack of alternative response options, and (iii) received small rewards for the correct actions ([1,2,4,5], but see [6,7,8]). These conditions markedly contrast with real-world human economic behaviours that offer many different formats and variants, often occur over extended periods of time, are spontaneously engaged in by a very heterogeneous population, use a range of symbolic currencies and are influenced by a rich social context [3,9].

In this respect, the external and ecological validities of the currently available token exchange paradigm could be put into question. This is not to say that the results obtained from the current studies suffer from a complete lack of validity, or that the results may not be informative about some contexts. However, the actual impact of conducting these experiments in these artificial conditions is unknown and needs to be investigated. One way to do that is to study more externally and ecologically valid systems of economic behaviours in non-human primates, and then to critically examine the generalizability of findings from laboratory models. This approach should provide a more solid platform for conducting comparative economics research and shed light on the evolution of human monetary systems [3].

Around the Uluwatu Temple in Bali, Indonesia, a large free-ranging population of long-tailed macaques (Macaca fascicularis) spontaneously and routinely engage in token-mediated bartering interactions with humans. These interactions occur in two phases: after stealing inedible and more or less valuable objects (e.g. pairs of glasses, hats, empty bags) from temple visitors, the macaques appear to use them as tokens, by returning them to humans in exchange for a certain number/type of food rewards proffered by the temple staff [10]. In many respects, these naturally occurring token-robbing and token/reward-bartering interactions are reminiscent of the token exchange paradigm experimentally implemented by researchers in captive settings, in which the symbolic value of a token lies in the quantity and quality of the food reward gained in return [2,8,9,10,11,12,13,14,15]. Our research project is the first to explore the cognitive and behavioural mechanisms underlying the spontaneous expression of token-mediated bartering interactions in naturalistic circumstances (i.e. the macaques initiate token-robbing interactions without any encouragement from humans). These interactions are at least partially monkey-driven (i.e. even though token/reward-bartering interactions depend on the willingness of human barterers to exchange, the macaques can choose to barter or not), and exhibited by a large number of free-ranging individuals [10].

Recent reports on robbing/bartering in the Uluwatu macaques have shown that this behaviour was (i) population-specific, because it was not observed in other anthropogenically influenced populations of Balinese macaques where it would be ecologically possible, (ii) frequent (7.8 robbing events/hour and 3.3 bartering events/hour), (iii) prevalent (49.3% of identified population members performed robbing behaviour and 35.6% performed both robbing and bartering behaviours), (iv) performed by individuals from all age/sex classes in the five neighbouring social groups of this population, (v) characterized by substantial intrapopulational intergroup variation in its frequency and prevalence (but not success), which was explained by anthropogenic influences (i.e. differential environmental opportunities to interact with temple visitors), and to a lesser extent, demographic correlates (i.e. the behaviour was more frequent and more prevalent in groups with higher ratios of (sub)adult males, but not group density), (vi) a behavioural candidate for the process of ‘cultural zones' (in which neighbouring social groups share space, information and behavioural practices) via intergroup observational learning or intergroup transfers of male group members, (vii) socially influenced and synchronized in its group-level expression, following a behavioural contagion-like effect, owing to response facilitation and possibly conformity-biased learning, (viii) not influenced by model-based biases (i.e. dominance rank, age, experience and success of the demonstrator), and (ix) an established practice in this population, probably passed on cross-generationally for at least 30 years, though detailed information on its origins is lacking [10,16]. However, we do not know whether this behaviour is learned through individual practice (i.e. via experiential trial-and-error learning) and underlain by economic decision-making processes—that is, subjected to robbing/bartering payoff maximization as would be suggested by clear behavioural associations between value-based token possession and quantity or quality of food rewards rejected and accepted by the monkeys.

This study aimed to fill this knowledge gap by testing three hypotheses pertaining to the acquisition and skillful performance of robbing and bartering interactions in the Uluwatu population of long-tailed macaques. First, the ‘experiential learning' hypothesis posits age differences in robbing/bartering success. Indeed, each of the two aforementioned phases (i.e. robbing and bartering) comprises several successive behavioural steps or strategies that must all be appropriately completed by the monkey for the phase to be considered successful. Both token-robbing and token/reward-bartering are challenging tasks for a monkey and require specific perceptual learning, sensorimotor coordination and cognitive skills. From juveniles to subadults to adults, we predicted (i) an increase in token-robbing success (i.e. the monkey sequentially stared at a temple visitor, approached and snatched a token from this person, and held the token, while stepping aside; Prediction 1a), (ii) an increase in token/reward-bartering success (i.e. the monkey waited for a human barterer and returned the undamaged token in exchange for at least one food reward; Prediction 1b), and (iii) more negotiated successful token/reward-bartering sequences (i.e. the monkey only returned the token after being proposed more food rewards, or after rejecting more food rewards, or after accepting a type of food reward different from the one(s) previously rejected; Prediction 1c).

Second, assuming a hierarchical scale of values attributed by humans to different tokens (with more valuable tokens leading to more bartering attempts by the temple staff), the ‘value-based token selection' hypothesis posits age differences in the selection of higher-valued tokens by the monkeys during the token-robbing phase. From juveniles to subadults to adults, we predicted an increase in the relative selection of higher-valued tokens available among all the temple visitors present on the site (Prediction 2a). When two tokens of different values were available on a given temple visitor, we predicted that subadult and adult monkeys would preferentially select the higher-valued token, whereas no significant difference was expected in juvenile monkeys (Prediction 2b). We predicted a similar age difference in the value-based token selection during field experiments, after controlling for token accessibility and lateral bias (Prediction 2c).

Third, assuming that economic decision-making requires cognitive maturity, the ‘robbing/bartering payoff maximization' hypothesis posits clear behavioural associations between value-based token possession and quantity or quality of food rewards rejected and accepted by subadult and adult monkeys (i.e. hypothetically the most skillful and selective individuals) during the token/reward-bartering phase. In other words, these individuals should respond differently to different token values, and use the token/reward-bartering phase to obtain either more food rewards or a more preferred food reward when they selected a higher-valued token. In terms of reward quantity, subadult and adult subjects should (i) wait to be proposed more food rewards (Prediction 3a), and (ii) accumulate more than one of the proposed food rewards (Prediction 3b) before returning the token when it is a higher-valued token. In terms of reward quality, they should (i) reject more low-preferred food rewards before returning the token when it is a higher-valued token (Prediction 3c) and (ii) more likely end a successful token/reward-bartering sequence by returning the token in exchange for a type of food reward different from the one(s) previously rejected when it is a higher-valued token (Prediction 3d). Conversely, subadult and adult subjects should more likely end a successful token/reward-bartering sequence by returning the token in exchange for a low-preferred food reward when it is a lower-valued token (Prediction 3e).

2. Methods

The Uluwatu Temple, located in a dry agricultural landscape of southern Bali, Indonesia, is a Hindu temple complex used by Balinese communities for daily religious ceremonies. It is also one of the most famous touristic spots on the island, visited by 1.5 million tourists in 2015 [16]. A population of long-tailed macaques has lived in this anthropogenic habitat for decades [17]. In April 2016, the study population comprised 333 individuals (55 adult males (older than 6 years), 21 subadult males (4–6 years), 79 adult females (older than 3.5 years), 20 subadult females (2.5–3.5 years), 94 juveniles (1–4 years for males and 1–2.5 for females) and 64 infants (younger than 1 year) split up into five neighbouring social groups with overlapping home ranges [16]. The Uluwatu macaques were provisioned daily with a variety of fruits and vegetables provided by the temple staff, and fully habituated to human presence. Overall, this habitat is a human-dominated area characterized by highly frequent interactions with (local and tourist) people and a very low predation pressure [16].

Observational data were based on the spontaneous expression of token-robbing and token/reward-bartering interactions between monkeys and humans, with no intervention from the researchers. These data were collected by using the combined ‘behaviour-dependent sampling' rule and the ‘continuous recording' rule [18]. Behaviour-dependent sampling typically applies to data collection on ‘conspicuous' or ‘attention-attracting' behaviours [18, p.87]; these are characteristics that fit token-robbing and token/reward-bartering interactions, particularly because they usually occur within a few wide and open areas located around Uluwatu temple, where visitors are allowed and visibility for the researchers was good [10,16]. All observational data were continuously video-recorded from September 2015 to August 2016 (over 273 observation days) by two observers, using handheld camcorders (Sony HDR-PJ670). They typically walked across these open areas, regularly scanned the monkeys and their potential human targets around them, and started video-recording when a monkey stared at a prospective human target while approaching this person within 5 m. A prospective human target was defined as a temple visitor wearing or carrying around at least one inedible object that was more or less likely to be subsequently exchanged for food if stolen; examples of worn tokens included sun/eyeglasses and flip-flops, whereas examples of tokens carried around included cell phones and empty camera bags.

For each robbing/bartering event, one of the two observers video-recorded the complete token-robbing attempt and token/reward-bartering attempt, including the identity of the monkey being involved, the object (i.e. prospective token) being selected by the monkey, the number and type of other potential tokens directly accessible to the monkey (i.e. held or carried around by the human target), and the number and type of food reward(s) being (i) proposed/tossed to the monkey by an accustomed temple staff member typically located within 3 metres, (ii) ignored/rejected by the monkey with a ‘turning down' hand/arm gesture, and (iii) accepted/accumulated by the monkey by grabbing/cuddling the food reward(s). Because the monkeys were highly habituated to humans, most video-recordings were collected at close range (2–5 m) without disturbing the animals. Data collection started when the two observers reached high inter-observer reliability, as measured by the index of concordance (C) for monkey identities (N = 390 samples, C = 0.96) and age–sex class identification (N = 350, C = 0.93), and distances in metres (N = 200, C = 0.91) [16,19].

Even though these environmental conditions and behavioural sampling techniques allow for a majority of the token-robbing and token/reward-bartering interactions to be accurately and consistently recorded from start to finish, they did not provide absolute behavioural frequencies (i.e. number of events/behaviours per observation time unit) because the study site was too large for all robbing/bartering occurrences to be recorded at any time. However, this data collection method allowed us to quantify all the other variables used in this study (i.e. individual percentages of robbing/bartering successful attempts; number and type of object(s) being available on, and selected from, a human target; individual average numbers of food rewards being proposed, rejected, and accepted) because these measures were based on the relative behavioural frequencies calculated from a total of recorded events. All these variables were obtained from video-scoring. This task was conducted by N.G., who transcribed all the video-recorded robbing/bartering interactions onto an Excel spreadsheet, with each aforementioned variable being coded to the second. To measure intra-coder reliability, N.G. transcribed twice a total of 6.2 h of video-recordings, involving 158 token-robbing attempts and 66 token/reward-bartering attempts, which represents 3% of the total numbers of token-robbing attempts and token/reward-bartering attempts included in the analyses of token-robbing success and token/reward-bartering success, respectively. The comparison of the two transcriptions for all the other variables analyzed in this study yielded a high score of coder consistency (mean Cohen's κ coefficient: k = 0.89 ± 0.05 [19]).

We used individual percentages of successful token-robbing attempts and successful token/reward-bartering attempts as valid measures of experiential learning by conducting cross-sectional analyses based on age differences. To calculate these percentages, we used each monkey subject as its own control by distinguishing for each attempt two possible outcomes—successful or unsuccessful—out of the total number of attempts for each subject.

There was a four-stepped sequence of appropriate behaviours and environmental conditions leading to a successful token-robbing outcome: the monkey (i) stared at a prospective human target, (ii) inconspicuously approached the human target within 5 m, (iii) snatched the object from the human target (monkey–human body contact may or may not occur), and (iv) stepped aside while gripping the token. If any of these behavioural steps was not appropriately completed by the monkey, the token-robbing attempt was considered unsuccessful. To be included in the analyses of token-robbing outcome, an individual had to be involved in at least four token-robbing attempts (mean number of token-robbing attempts/individual ±s.d. = 53.5 ± 76.9). We believe that this minimum number of four attempts per individual made the distinction between unsuccessful and successful outcomes valid, while allowing us to reach a sample size for the ‘juvenile' age class that was conducive to statistical analysis.

Following a successful token-robbing attempt, there was a three-stepped sequence of appropriate behaviours and environmental conditions leading to a successful token/reward-bartering outcome: the monkey (i) held onto the stolen object and remained present in the area for a potential token/reward-bartering attempt involving a human barterer (i.e. typically one of the accustomed temple staff members, or occasionally the human target), instead of fleeing with an object that only acquires a value for the monkey during the bartering process, (ii) engaged in a token/reward-bartering interaction, which may involve accumulating several food rewards before returning the token, or even discarding a (a number of) less preferred food reward(s) in anticipation of receiving a (number of) more preferred food reward(s), and (iii) refrained itself from damaging the token and returned it in good condition, because if the token was damaged by the monkey, the bartering interaction may not occur or may be interrupted. If any of these behavioural steps was not appropriately completed by the monkey, the token/reward-bartering attempt was considered unsuccessful. To be included in the analyses of token/reward-bartering success, an individual had to be involved in at least four token/reward-bartering attempts (mean number of token/reward-bartering attempts/individual ± s.d. = 31.6 ± 39.0).

Our observational data showed six types of tokens selected (i.e. either missed, if the token-robbing attempt was unsuccessful, or taken, if it was successful) by the Uluwatu monkeys: (i) empty containers (e.g. phone cases, camera bags, plastic bottles; Nmissed = 260, Ntaken = 287); (ii) accessories (e.g. hairpins, keyrings, bag-charms; Nmissed = 171, Ntaken = 270); (iii) hats (e.g. headgear, caps, crowns, veils, scarves; Nmissed = 181, Ntaken = 257); (iv) shoes (e.g. flip-flops, heeled sandals; Nmissed = 897, Ntaken = 902); (v) pairs of glasses (e.g. eyeglasses, sunglasses; Nmissed = 351, Ntaken = 1773); and (vi) electronic devices/wallets (e.g. cell phones, cameras, tablets, purses; Nmissed = 48, Ntaken = 77). Edible objects selected by the monkeys were not considered as tokens in our analyses.

To establish a hierarchical scale of values assigned by humans to these six types of tokens, we ranked them by increasing percentages of successful token-robbing attempts leading to token/reward-bartering attempts by humans (i.e. assuming more human-valued tokens should lead to more token/reward-bartering attempts by humans to retrieve the tokens from the monkeys). Our observational data showed three groups (each containing two types) of value-based tokens on the basis of how often humans wanted to engage in token/reward-bartering interactions with the monkeys: (i) low-valued tokens, seldom bartered by humans: empty containers and accessories (leading to bartering attempts in 8.4% and 16.3% of events, respectively); (ii) medium-valued tokens, often bartered by humans: hats and shoes (leading to bartering attempts in 74.3% and 75.8% of events, respectively); and (iii) high-valued tokens, almost always bartered by humans: pairs of glasses and electronic devices/wallets (leading to bartering attempts in 90.6% and 98.7% of events, respectively).

To assess the relative local availability in the six different types of tokens, we examined a sample of 84 video-recorded token-robbing events, randomly selected from our observational data, that featured 500 potential human targets. For each of these potential human targets, we scored the number and the type of potential tokens directly accessible to the monkeys. From this representative sample, the total number of available tokens we obtained was N = 1084, distributed across the six types of tokens as follows: (i) 127 empty containers; (ii) 107 accessories; (iii) 132 hats; (iv) 466 (pairs of) shoes; (v) 120 pairs of glasses; and (vi) 132 electronic devices/wallets. To assess the relative ease with which the six different types of tokens were stolen by the monkeys from human targets, we calculated, for each type of token, the average percentage of successful token-robbing attempts among all the subadult and adult monkeys (i.e. the most skilled robbers) that had each been involved in at least four token-robbing attempts targeting a given type of token in our observational dataset.

Experimental data were obtained from value-based token selection tests administered to a subset of 15 monkeys belonging to the three age classes (i.e. five juvenile, five subadult and five adult individuals) by three experimenters between September and December 2019. When each of these subjects was not surrounded by other monkeys, it was presented with a binary choice between a medium-valued token and a high-valued token, each proffered at opposing sides of a black plastic tray (30 cm × 20 cm × 4 cm). The tray was placed on the ground at 1.2 m from the tested monkey. The preferentially selected token was deemed to be the one the subject (first) grasped/took possession of. Each subject was tested across 16 trials comprising four randomly rotated sessions of four randomly rotated binary choices between a medium-valued token and a high-valued token (i.e. four trials with hat versus pairs of glasses, four trials with hat versus phone, four trials with shoe versus pairs of glasses and four trials with shoe versus phone). To control for lateral biases in grasping behaviour, token orientation (i.e. left/right) was balanced across trials. To control for experimenter biases, the experimenters fixed their gaze at a fixed point away from the proffered tokens during each trial. Most of these trials were followed by token/reward-bartering attempts by experimenters to retrieve the tokens. In terms of timing, and as much as possible considering field study constraints, the trials were homogeneously distributed within and across subjects, with no more than five trials being performed on a given subject within a day. Experimental data collection started when the three experimenters reached 100% of concordance for monkey identities.

For analyses on food rewards rejected and accepted by the monkeys during the token/reward-bartering phase, we only considered the food items that the monkey could reach/grab when proposed/tossed to them by the human barterers. To assess individual preferences across the three types of proposed food rewards (i.e. a raw egg, a small plastic bag containing a few pieces of fruit and a cracker), we used our observational data on spontaneously expressed token/reward-bartering interactions.

For a given monkey, we calculated three ‘food reward likeability' scores, one for each of the three types of proposed food rewards. When the three reward types were proposed during a token/reward-bartering sequence, we used two values: X was defined as the total number of times a certain reward type was the last one accepted by the monkey before returning the token and Y was defined as the total number of times this reward type was rejected by the monkey during token/reward-bartering sequences. The individual ‘likeability' score for a reward type was obtained by dividing X by (X + Y). The least-preferred type of food rewards for a given monkey was considered to be the one with the lowest ‘likeability' score.

Because all our raw and (log, square-root and arsine-square-root) transformed data violated parametric assumptions, we conducted nonparametric tests. To test age differences in the percentage of successful token-robbing attempts, the percentage of successful token/reward-bartering attempts, and the average number of food rewards being proposed and rejected by the monkey, we used Kruskal–Wallis H-tests followed, when significant, by post hoc Mann–Whitney U-tests. To test whether the monkeys selected the six different types of tokens based on their relative local availability, we used a Spearman rank-order correlation test between the numbers of the six types of tokens available from our sample of 500 potential human targets and the numbers of the six types of tokens selected by the monkeys within our entire set of observational data (i.e. the sum of Nmissed and Ntaken for each type of token). To test whether the monkeys preferentially selected tokens that were easier to steal from humans, we used a Spearman rank-order correlation test between the percentage of types of tokens selected and the average percentage of successful token-robbing attempts among subadult and adult monkeys. To test within-individual preferential selection of low-valued, medium-valued and high-valued tokens, we used Friedman tests followed, when significant, by post hoc Wilcoxon signed-rank tests. To test within-individual preferential selection between two tokens of different values available on a given human target, we used Wilcoxon signed-rank tests. Value-based token selection tests of binary choices between a medium-valued token and a high-valued token were analyzed by using binomial tests with a test proportion = 0.50. Because all our predictions were directional, we conducted one-tailed tests. Statistical analyses were performed using the IBM-SPSS Statistics-26 analytical program. Significance levels were set at α = 0.05.

3. Results

The percentages of successful token-robbing attempts were 39.1% (N = 376 attempts), 61.5% (N = 1829 attempts) and 68.8% (N = 3062 attempts) in juveniles, subadults, and adults, respectively. We found a statistically significant age difference in the percentage of successful token-robbing attempts (Kruskal–Wallis test, H2 = 35.8, p < 0.001). Post hoc pairwise comparisons revealed that subadults were more successful than juveniles (Mann–Whitney test, Nsubadult = 20, Njuvenile = 17, U = 69.0, p = 0.002), adults were more successful than juveniles (Nadult = 60, U = 68.5, p < 0.001), and adults were more successful than subadults (U = 302.5, p = 0.001; figure 1). Therefore, we found a significant increase in token-robbing success from juveniles to subadults to adults; Prediction 1a was supported.

The percentages of successful token/reward-bartering attempts were 72.4% (N = 58 attempts), 89.4% (N = 689 attempts) and 92.0% (N = 1446 attempts) in juveniles, subadults and adults, respectively. We found a statistically significant age difference in the percentage of successful token/reward-bartering attempts (H2 = 22.3, p < 0.001). Post hoc pairwise comparisons revealed that subadults were more successful than juveniles (Nsubadult = 13, Njuvenile = 10, U = 9.5, p = 0.001) and adults were more successful than juveniles (Nadult = 45, U = 20.0, p < 0.001). However, adults and subadults did not significantly differ in percentages of successful token/reward-bartering attempts (U = 213.0, p = 0.135; figure 1). Even though we did not find a significant increase in token-robbing success from juveniles to subadults to adults, Prediction 1b was partly supported owing to the difference between the former and the latter two age classes.

During successful token/reward-bartering sequences, we found statistically significant age differences in the average number of food rewards being proposed to the monkey (H2 = 15.1, p = 0.001), the average number of food rewards being rejected by the monkey (H2 = 13.9, p = 0.001) and the average number of food rewards being rejected by the monkey before accepting a different type of food reward to end the token/reward-bartering sequence (H2 = 8.7, p = 0.013). In each of these three variables, subadults scored significantly higher than juveniles (Nsubadult = 14, Njuvenile = 9, U = 11.0, p < 0.001; U = 12.5, p = 0.001; U = 19.0, p = 0.004, respectively) and adults scored higher than juveniles (Nadult = 46, U = 41.5, p < 0.001; U = 49.0, p < 0.001; U = 87.5, p = 0.006, respectively). However, adults and subadults did not significantly differ in these scores (U = 301.0, p = 0.713; U = 313.5, p = 0.882; U = 305.0, p = 0.766, respectively). Even though we did not find a significant increase in the scores indicating negotiated successful token/reward-bartering sequences from juveniles to subadults to adults, Prediction 1c was partly supported owing to the difference between the former and the latter two age classes.

In the observational data, the relative local availability in the six types of tokens and the numbers of the six types of tokens selected by the monkeys within our entire set of observational data were not statistically correlated (Spearman's correlation coefficient, N = 6, Rs = −0.17, p = 0.742). Moreover, the average percentage of successful token-robbing attempts among all the subadult and adult monkeys (i.e. the most skilled robbers) and the percentage of types of tokens selected from human targets were not statistically correlated (Spearman's correlation coefficient, N = 6, Rs = −0.26, p = 0.623).

In the observational data, the percentages of selection of low-valued, medium-valued and high-valued tokens were, respectively, 25.3, 47.9 and 26.8% in juveniles, 18.8, 38.9 and 42.3% in subadults and 17.2, 41.4 and 41.4% in adults. We did not find any statistically significant difference in the selection of the three differentially valued tokens by juveniles (Friedman test, N = 17, χ2 = 4.9, p = 0.085). However, we found statistically significant differences in the selection of the three differentially valued tokens by subadults (N = 20, χ2 = 8.5, p = 0.014) and adults (N = 60, χ2 = 21.4, p < 0.001). Post hoc pairwise comparisons revealed that subadults and adults preferentially selected high-valued over low-valued tokens (Wilcoxon tests, subadults: z = −2.0, p = 0.049; adults: z = −3.6, p < 0.001) and medium-valued over low-valued tokens (subadults: z = −2.0, p = 0.049; adults: z = −2.6, p = 0.010). There was no significant difference in the selection of high-valued or medium-valued tokens in subadults (z = −0.2, p = 0.872) and adults (z = −0.1, p = 0.997). Even though we did not find a significant increase in the relative selection of higher-valued tokens among all the prospective human targets from juveniles to subadults to adults, Prediction 2a was partly supported owing to the difference between the former and the latter two age classes.

However, when two tokens of different values were available on a given human target, we found that subadult and adult monkeys preferentially selected the higher-valued token (Wilcoxon tests, Nsubadult = 12, z = −3.0, p = 0.003; Nadult = 12, z = −3.1, p = 0.002), whereas no significant difference was found in juvenile individuals (Njuvenile = 12, z = −0.775, p = 0.439). Therefore, Prediction 2b was supported.

In the experimental data, when two tokens of different values were available on plastic trays placed in front of a tested monkey, we found that subadult and adult subjects preferentially selected the higher-valued token (Binomial tests, subadults: Nmedium-valued tokens selected = 12, Nhigh-valued tokens selected = 68, p < 0.001; adults: Nmedium-valued tokens selected = 10, Nhigh-valued tokens selected = 70, p < 0.001), whereas no significant difference was found in juvenile subjects (Nmedium-valued tokens selected = 35, Nhigh-valued tokens selected = 45, p = 0.314). Therefore, Prediction 2c was supported.

In this section about economic decision-making (i.e. testing behavioural associations between value-based token possession and quantity or quality of food rewards rejected and accepted), we focused on the most skilful and selective individuals during the token/reward-bartering phase, namely, subadult and adult monkeys. In terms of reward quantity, subadult and adult subjects waited to be proposed more food rewards before returning the token when it was a higher-valued token (i.e. a high-valued rather than a medium-valued token; Wilcoxon tests, Nsubadult = 12, z = −3.1, p = 0.002; Nadult = 40, z = −3.0, p = 0.003). Low-valued tokens could not be used in these analyses because, by definition, they were typically not bartered by the temple staff. Still, Prediction 3a was supported.

We also found that the percentage of successful token/reward-bartering sequences when more than one of the proposed food rewards were accumulated by the monkey before returning the token was significantly higher when the monkey was holding a high-valued token than when it was holding a medium-valued token, both in subadult (Wilcoxon test, N = 10, z = −2.8, p = 0.005) and adult subjects (N = 29, z = −2.4, p = 0.015). Therefore, Prediction 3b was supported.

In terms of reward quality, and as expected, the average percentage of rejections, by adult subjects, of the least-preferred type of food rewards (when at least one reward among the least-preferred type was proposed during a given token/reward-bartering sequence) was significantly higher when they were holding a high-valued token, compared to a medium-valued token (Wilcoxon test, N = 22, z = −2.3, p = 0.024). However, this difference was not statistically significant in subadult subjects (Wilcoxon test, N = 10, z = −1.4, p = 0.153). Therefore, Prediction 3c was supported for adult, but not for subadult subjects.

Contrary to our expectations, there was no significant difference in the average number of food rewards being rejected by the monkey before accepting a different type of food reward to end the token/reward-bartering sequence when it was holding a high-valued token, compared to a medium-valued token, either in subadults (Wilcoxon test, N = 9, z = −1.2, p = 0.223) or in adults (Wilcoxon test, N = 29, z = −1.1, p = 0.280). Therefore, Prediction 3d was not supported.

Finally, subadult and adult subjects were significantly more likely to end a successful token/reward-bartering sequence by returning the token in exchange for the least-preferred type of food rewards (provided at least one reward among the least-preferred type was proposed during a given token/reward-bartering sequence) when they were holding a medium-valued token compared to when they were holding a high-valued token (Wilcoxon tests, Nsubadult = 10, z = −2.2, p = 0.028; Nadult = 22, z = −2.1, p = 0.033). Therefore, Prediction 3e was supported.

4. Discussion

This field observational and experimental study of token-robbing and token/reward-bartering interactions in the free-ranging population of Balinese long-tailed macaques produced three main findings: (i) these behaviours need to be learned throughout juvenescence (i.e. until up to 4 years in this species) to be successfully performed; (ii) older monkeys preferentially selected tokens that were more valued by humans; and (iii) these more skilful and selective individuals appeared to make economic decisions, as evidenced by clear behavioural associations between value-based token possession and quantity or quality of food rewards rejected and accepted.

As predicted, we found a significant increase in token-robbing success from juveniles to subadults to adults, whereas the main behaviour patterns required for the successful performance of token/reward-bartering interactions were already in place from around 4 years (i.e. in subadults). Likewise, the ability to engage in more negotiated successful token/reward-bartering sequences—during which the monkey only returned the token after being proposed more food rewards, or after rejecting more food rewards, or after accepting a type of food reward different from the one(s) previously rejected—was not fully acquired before the subadult stage.

These results lend some support to the ‘experiential learning&#x27; hypothesis, whereby token-robbing and token/reward-bartering interactions are multi-stepped and complex behavioural sequences requiring perceptual learning, sensorimotor coordination and cognitive skills (e.g. memory, associative learning) to be successfully performed; they are thus gradually acquired through extended individual practice during the juvenile period, in part via experiential trial-and-error learning. It is noteworthy to mention that the development of (sub)adult-level proficiency at robbing/bartering is not only dependent on skill learning (e.g. detection, sneaky approach, self-control), but may also be constrained by physical maturation. This is particularly true during the token-robbing phase that often involves monkey–human body contact and/or requires muscular strength when a monkey has to yank on a flip-flop still worn by an adult human. In these cases, the limited physical capabilities of juveniles, and the maturing bodies of subadults, may partly explain the significant increase in token-robbing success from juveniles to subadults to adults.

Primates are characterized by the longest juvenile period in relation to life span of all mammals [20]. According to the ‘needing-to-learn' hypothesis [20], prolonged juvenility is associated with behavioural patterns that necessitate acquiring a proportionally large amount of information and/or skills to reach adult competence before individuals become reproductively mature. These behaviours include extractive foraging techniques [21] and (socio-)sexual behaviour patterns [22]. Our study indicates that both phases of the robbing/bartering practice also required experiential learning to be fully mastered.

The first step in economic decision-making requires the cognitive ability to distinguish among different expected material values of a given symbolic currency (e.g. tokens, cash, virtual money). After showing that token selection was not significantly affected by token availability and the relative ease with which different types of tokens were stolen by the monkeys from human targets, our observational data revealed a marked age difference in how the monkeys responded to a human-based three-level hierarchy of valuable objects. When considering the token selection among all the prospective human targets (i.e. temple visitors with potential tokens available in a given area), juveniles did not show any preferential selection among low-valued, medium-valued and high-valued tokens, whereas subadults and adults preferentially selected high- and medium-valued tokens over low-valued ones. When two tokens of different values were available on a given human target, subadults and adults preferentially selected the higher-value token, whereas juvenile individuals did not show any significant difference. We found a similar age difference in the value-based token selection, after experimentally controlling for token accessibility and lateral bias.

These results support the ‘value-based token selection' hypothesis, positing age differences in the selection of higher-valued tokens by the monkeys during the token-robbing phase that are indicative of a developmental trajectory toward more strategic choices in more mature individuals. Subadult and adult monkeys (but not juveniles yet) have learned to map their token-robbing behaviours onto the hierarchical (and arbitrary) scale of values attributed by humans to different tokens: they preferentially selected tokens that were more likely to be exchanged for food (e.g. electronic devices, pairs of glasses) over other objects that were less valuable for humans and typically not worth bartering (e.g. empty camera bags, hairpins). Our findings are consistent with data obtained in other non-human primate species, showing that subadult and adult capuchin monkeys and chimpanzees correctly preferred a high-valued token over a low-valued token in an experimental bartering situation [1,11,23].

The second step in economic decision-making requires the cognitive ability (i.e. mental processes involving associative learning and memorization) to respond differently to differentially valued tokens by trying to maximize one's payoff. We found evidence for such behavioural associations between value-based token possession and quantity or quality of food rewards rejected and accepted by subadult and adult monkeys (i.e. the most skilful and selective individuals) during the token/reward-bartering phase. They consistently and actively obtained either more food rewards or a more preferred food reward in exchange for a higher-valued token. They were also more likely to end a successful bartering interaction by accepting a less preferred food reward in exchange for a lower-valued token.

Our findings support the ‘robbing/bartering payoff maximization' hypothesis in that subadult and adult monkeys strategically responded to differentially valued tokens in their possession by adjusting the amount or type of food rewards they gained from the barters. The result showing that subadults (unlike adults) failed to significantly reject more low-preferred food rewards before returning a higher-valued token may be explained in terms of poorer temporal cognition or higher impulsivity (compared to adults): subadult long-tailed macaques may either not have yet acquired the cognitive capacity to anticipate the subsequent proffering of more preferred food rewards in this specific situation, or not be patient/self-controlled enough to wait for possibly more preferred food rewards.

Overall, our field observational data are in line with laboratory-based studies showing that several non-human primate species can (i) understand the effectiveness of tokens as secondary reinforcements to make simple calculations about quantities of reward, (ii) determine an item's value on the basis of its perceived utility (e.g. exchanging only a low-preferred reward for a tool necessary to reach a more preferred reward) and (iii) recognize the appropriate conditions in which a successful exchange could occur (e.g. presence/absence of the experimenter, safe/risky experimenter) [1,2,23,24,25,26,27]. Other cognitive skills and temperamental traits exhibited to varying extents by non-human primates engaging in token-aided economic behaviours include preference transitivity, self-control, delay of gratification, action planning and calculated reciprocity, because they may facilitate or constrain an individual's ability to make optimal economic decisions [1,5,23,28]. Even though these characteristics were not explicitly examined in this study, some of them will be the subject of our future observational and experimental investigations.

5. Conclusion

Token-robbing and token/reward-bartering are cognitively challenging tasks for the Uluwatu macaques that revealed unprecedented economic decision-making processes (i.e. valued-based token selection and payoff maximization) in a large monkey population living in an anthropogenically impacted habitat. This spontaneous, population-specific, prevalent, cross-generational, learned and socially influenced practice may be the first example of a culturally maintained token economy in free-ranging animals. The present naturalistic research setting represents a unique opportunity to study field economics and explore macroeconomic phenomena in non-human primates in environmental conditions that are more externally and ecologically valid than those provided by the traditional token exchange paradigm. Further experimental research on the Uluwatu macaques should make future cross-species comparisons of economic decision-making and symbolic tool use more relevant from an evolutionary perspective and may ultimately lead to a better understanding of the origins of autonomous monetary systems in humans [29].

The research protocol of this study was approved by the Animal Welfare Committee of The University of Lethbridge (Alberta, Canada) (Protocol #1430). Our study was conducted with research permission from the Indonesian Ministry of Research and Technology (#328/SIP/FRP/E5/Dit.KI/IX/2015; #410/SIP/FRP/E5/Dit.KI/X/2015), related Indonesian government and provincial agencies and the local authorities of the Uluwatu Temple.

Data accessibility

The dataset ‘Acquisition of object-robbing and object/food-bartering behaviours: a culturally maintained token economy in free-ranging long-tailed macaques’ is available from the Dryad Digital Repository: https://doi.org/10.5061/dryad.wpzgmsbjh [30].

J.-B.L. contributed to research design, data analysis and manuscript drafting. N.G. conducted video data scoring and contributed to research design, data analysis and manuscript drafting. M.G. contributed to research design, data collection, data analysis and manuscript drafting. I.N.W. assisted with research permission and contributed to research design. All authors gave final approval for publication and agreed to be held accountable for the work performed therein.

Competing interests

We declare we have no competing interests.

Funding

For this study, J.-B.L. was funded by a Discovery Grant from the Natural Sciences and Engineering Research Council of Canada (2015-06034), a Major Grant from the Alberta Gambling Research Institute (43699-4200-8001), a research allowance from the Office of the Dean of Arts and Science at the University of Lethbridge (14460-4200-8015) and a University of Lethbridge Research Fund (13424-4200-8015).

Acknowledgements

We thank Fany Brotcorne, Caleb Bunselmeyer, Christian Dunn, and Anna Holzner for assistance with data collection. We thank the Indonesian Ministry of Research and Technology and the Uluwatu Temple management committee for permission to conduct this research in Indonesia. We thank two anonymous reviewers for fruitful comments on a previous version of the manuscript.

One contribution of 17 to a theme issue ‘Existence and prevalence of economic behaviours among non-human primates’.

Published by the Royal Society under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/, which permits unrestricted use, provided the original author and source are credited.

References
https://openalex.org/W1210438732
https://openalex.org/W1965709685
https://openalex.org/W1967907348
https://openalex.org/W1972903509
https://openalex.org/W1985067115
https://openalex.org/W1987998509
https://openalex.org/W1993090068
https://openalex.org/W1997807823
https://openalex.org/W2009542006
https://openalex.org/W2015876871
https://openalex.org/W2028008698
https://openalex.org/W2038222114
https://openalex.org/W2052880680
https://openalex.org/W2053154970
https://openalex.org/W2099984914
https://openalex.org/W2100164632
https://openalex.org/W2102234092
https://openalex.org/W2103706356
https://openalex.org/W2110554592
https://openalex.org/W2128460088
https://openalex.org/W2142107967
https://openalex.org/W2149071596
https://openalex.org/W2165474269
https://openalex.org/W2615418517
https://openalex.org/W2907023993
https://openalex.org/W2995027344
https://openalex.org/W2996203231
https://openalex.org/W4240198423
https://openalex.org/W563084280